• More from M-W
  • To save this word, you'll need to log in. Log In

Definition of represent

 (Entry 1 of 2)

transitive verb

intransitive verb

Definition of re-present  (Entry 2 of 2)

  • characterize

Examples of represent in a Sentence

These examples are programmatically compiled from various online sources to illustrate current usage of the word 'represent.' Any opinions expressed in the examples do not represent those of Merriam-Webster or its editors. Send us feedback about these examples.

Word History

Middle English, from Anglo-French representer , from Latin repraesentare , from re- + praesentare to present

14th century, in the meaning defined at transitive sense 1

1564, in the meaning defined above

Dictionary Entries Near represent

reprehensory

Cite this Entry

“Represent.” Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary , Merriam-Webster, https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/represent. Accessed 23 Apr. 2024.

Kids Definition

Kids definition of represent, legal definition, legal definition of represent, more from merriam-webster on represent.

Nglish: Translation of represent for Spanish Speakers

Britannica English: Translation of represent for Arabic Speakers

Subscribe to America's largest dictionary and get thousands more definitions and advanced search—ad free!

Play Quordle: Guess all four words in a limited number of tries.  Each of your guesses must be a real 5-letter word.

Can you solve 4 words at once?

Word of the day.

See Definitions and Examples »

Get Word of the Day daily email!

Popular in Grammar & Usage

Commonly misspelled words, how to use em dashes (—), en dashes (–) , and hyphens (-), absent letters that are heard anyway, how to use accents and diacritical marks, on 'biweekly' and 'bimonthly', popular in wordplay, the words of the week - apr. 19, 10 words from taylor swift songs (merriam's version), a great big list of bread words, 9 superb owl words, 10 words for lesser-known games and sports, games & quizzes.

Play Blossom: Solve today's spelling word game by finding as many words as you can using just 7 letters. Longer words score more points.

  • Daily Crossword
  • Word Puzzle
  • Word Finder

Word of the Day

  • Synonym of the Day
  • Word of the Year
  • Language stories
  • All featured
  • Gender and sexuality
  • All pop culture
  • Grammar Coach ™
  • Writing hub
  • Grammar essentials
  • Commonly confused
  • All writing tips
  • Pop culture
  • Writing tips

Advertisement

representation

[ rep-ri-zen- tey -sh uh n , -z uh n- ]

  • the act of representing.
  • the state of being represented.
  • the expression or designation by some term, character, symbol, or the like.
  • action or speech on behalf of a person, group, business house, state, or the like by an agent, deputy, or representative.

to demand representation on a board of directors.

  • Government. the state, fact, or right of being represented by delegates having a voice in legislation or government.
  • the body or number of representatives, as of a constituency.
  • the act of speaking or negotiating on behalf of a state.
  • an utterance on behalf of a state.
  • presentation to the mind, as of an idea or image.
  • a mental image or idea so presented; concept.
  • the act of portrayal, picturing, or other rendering in visible form.
  • a picture, figure, statue, etc.
  • the production or a performance of a play or the like, as on the stage.
  • Often representations. a description or statement, as of things true or alleged.
  • a statement of facts, reasons, etc., made in appealing or protesting; a protest or remonstrance.

a representation of authority.

/ ˌrɛprɪzɛnˈteɪʃən /

  • the act or an instance of representing or the state of being represented
  • anything that represents, such as a verbal or pictorial portrait
  • anything that is represented, such as an image brought clearly to mind
  • the principle by which delegates act for a constituency
  • a body of representatives
  • contract law a statement of fact made by one party to induce another to enter into a contract
  • an instance of acting for another, on his authority, in a particular capacity, such as executor or administrator
  • a dramatic production or performance
  • often plural a statement of facts, true or alleged, esp one set forth by way of remonstrance or expostulation

phonetic representation

Discover More

Other words from.

  • nonrep·re·sen·tation noun
  • over·repre·sen·tation noun
  • prerep·re·sen·tation noun
  • self-repre·sen·tation noun
  • under·repre·sen·tation noun

Word History and Origins

Origin of representation 1

Example Sentences

It was a metaphorical statement of giving and withdrawing consent for a show rooted in a literal representation of Coel being assaulted.

The mathematically manipulated results are passed on and augmented through the stages, finally producing an integrated representation of a face.

I hope this list—a representation of the most consequential changes taking places in our world—is similarly useful for you.

“Given the moment we are in, I can only hope our institutions really understand what this failure of representation means to our city,” he said.

The voters don’t want to have an elected city attorney on the, and representation said, that’s fine.

With all that said, representation of each of these respective communities has increased in the new Congress.

As this excellent piece in Mother Jones describes, however, Holsey had outrageously poor representation during his trial.

During that time days, Livvix went through court hearings without legal representation.

What do you think prompted the change in comic book representation of LGBTQ characters?

Barbie is an unrealistic, unhealthy, insulting representation of female appearance.

With less intelligent children traces of this tendency to take pictorial representation for reality may appear as late as four.

As observation widens and grows finer, the first bald representation becomes fuller and more life-like.

The child now aims at constructing a particular linear representation, that of a man, a horse, or what not.

He had heard it hinted that allowing the colonies representation in Parliament would be a simple plan for making taxes legal.

But sufficient can be discerned for the grasping of the idea, which seems to be a representation of the Nativity.

Related Words

[ ak -s uh -lot-l ]

Start each day with the Word of the Day in your inbox!

By clicking "Sign Up", you are accepting Dictionary.com Terms & Conditions and Privacy Policies.

Cambridge Dictionary

  • Cambridge Dictionary +Plus

Meaning of representation in English

Your browser doesn't support HTML5 audio

representation noun ( ACTING FOR )

  • Defendants have a right to legal representation and must be informed of that right when they are arrested .
  • The farmers demanded greater representation in parliament .
  • The main opposing parties have nearly equal representation in the legislature .
  • The scheme is intended to increase representation of minority groups .
  • The members are chosen by a system of proportional representation.
  • admissibility
  • extinguishment
  • extrajudicial
  • extrajudicially
  • out-of-court
  • pay damages
  • plea bargain
  • walk free idiom

representation noun ( DESCRIPTION )

  • anti-realism
  • anti-realist
  • complementary
  • confederate
  • naturalistically
  • non-figurative
  • non-representational
  • poetic license
  • symbolization

representation noun ( INCLUDING ALL )

  • all manner of something idiom
  • alphabet soup
  • it takes all sorts (to make a world) idiom
  • non-segregated
  • odds and ends
  • of every stripe/of all stripes idiom
  • this and that idiom
  • variety is the spice of life idiom
  • wide choice

representation | Business English

Examples of representation, collocations with representation.

  • representation

These are words often used in combination with representation .

Click on a collocation to see more examples of it.

Translations of representation

Get a quick, free translation!

{{randomImageQuizHook.quizId}}

Word of the Day

rescue centre

a place where animals who are ill, injured, not cared for, or badly treated can be taken and given treatment and care

Binding, nailing, and gluing: talking about fastening things together

Binding, nailing, and gluing: talking about fastening things together

representation meaning verb

Learn more with +Plus

  • Recent and Recommended {{#preferredDictionaries}} {{name}} {{/preferredDictionaries}}
  • Definitions Clear explanations of natural written and spoken English English Learner’s Dictionary Essential British English Essential American English
  • Grammar and thesaurus Usage explanations of natural written and spoken English Grammar Thesaurus
  • Pronunciation British and American pronunciations with audio English Pronunciation
  • English–Chinese (Simplified) Chinese (Simplified)–English
  • English–Chinese (Traditional) Chinese (Traditional)–English
  • English–Dutch Dutch–English
  • English–French French–English
  • English–German German–English
  • English–Indonesian Indonesian–English
  • English–Italian Italian–English
  • English–Japanese Japanese–English
  • English–Norwegian Norwegian–English
  • English–Polish Polish–English
  • English–Portuguese Portuguese–English
  • English–Spanish Spanish–English
  • English–Swedish Swedish–English
  • Dictionary +Plus Word Lists
  • representation (ACTING FOR)
  • representation (DESCRIPTION)
  • representation (INCLUDING ALL)
  • make representations to sb
  • Collocations
  • Translations
  • All translations

Add representation to one of your lists below, or create a new one.

{{message}}

Something went wrong.

There was a problem sending your report.

  • Dictionaries home
  • American English
  • Collocations
  • German-English
  • Grammar home
  • Practical English Usage
  • Learn & Practise Grammar (Beta)
  • Word Lists home
  • My Word Lists
  • Recent additions
  • Resources home
  • Text Checker

Definition of representation noun from the Oxford Advanced American Dictionary

representation

Want to learn more?

Find out which words work together and produce more natural-sounding English with the Oxford Collocations Dictionary app. Try it for free as part of the Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary app.

representation meaning verb

  • Previous Article

Mental Representation of Verb Meaning: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence

* Current address: Southeastern University, Nanjing, China

  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions
  • Article contents
  • Figures & tables
  • Supplementary Data
  • Peer Review
  • Search Site

Xuesong Li , Hua Shu , Youyi Liu , Ping Li; Mental Representation of Verb Meaning: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence. J Cogn Neurosci 2006; 18 (10): 1774–1787. doi: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1774

Download citation file:

  • Ris (Zotero)
  • Reference Manager

Previous psycholinguistic research has debated the nature of the mental representation of verbs and the access of relevant verb information in sentence processing. In this study, we used behavioral and electrophysiological methods to examine the representation of verbs in and out of sentence contexts. In five experiments, word naming and event-related potential (ERP) components were used to measure the speed and the amplitude, respectively, associated with different verb-object combinations that result in different degrees of fit between the verb and its object. Both naming speed and ERP amplitudes (N400) are proven to be sensitive indices of the degree of fit, varying as a function of how well the object fits the verb in terms of selectional restrictions. The results suggest that the semantic features of the verb's arguments are an integral part of the mental representation of verbs, and such information of the verb is accessed and used on-line during sentence processing. Implications of these results are discussed in light of recent computational semantic models that view the lexicon through high-order lexical co-occurrences in language use.

Client Account

Sign in via your institution, email alerts, related articles, related book chapters, affiliations.

  • Online ISSN 1530-8898
  • Print ISSN 0898-929X

A product of The MIT Press

Mit press direct.

  • About MIT Press Direct

Information

  • Accessibility
  • For Authors
  • For Customers
  • For Librarians
  • Direct to Open
  • Open Access
  • Media Inquiries
  • Rights and Permissions
  • For Advertisers
  • About the MIT Press
  • The MIT Press Reader
  • MIT Press Blog
  • Seasonal Catalogs
  • MIT Press Home
  • Give to the MIT Press
  • Direct Service Desk
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Statement
  • Crossref Member
  • COUNTER Member  
  • The MIT Press colophon is registered in the U.S. Patent and Trademark Office

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Hum Brain Mapp
  • v.15(2); 2002 Feb

Logo of humanbrain

Neural representation of verb meaning: An fMRI study

Murray grossman.

1 Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

Phyllis Koenig

Chris devita, guila glosser, david alsop.

2 Department of Radiology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

The neural basis for verb comprehension has proven elusive, in part because of the limited range of verb categories that have been assessed. In the present study, 16 healthy young adults were probed for the meaning associated with verbs of MOTION and verbs of COGNITION. We observed distinct patterns of activation for each verb subcategory: MOTION verbs are associated with recruitment of left ventral temporal‐occipital cortex, bilateral prefrontal cortex and caudate, whereas COGNITION verbs are associated with left posterolateral temporal activation. These findings are consistent with the claim that the neural representations of verb subcategories are distinct. Although the “sensory‐motor” hypothesis may play a role in explaining activation associated with MOTION verbs, the left posterolateral temporal distribution of cortical activation associated with COGNITION verbs cannot be easily explained by the “sensory‐motor” hypothesis. We suggest that left posterolateral temporal activation supports aspects of lexical semantic processing concerned with the neural representation of propositional knowledge contributing to COGNITION verbs. Hum. Brain Mapping 15:124–134, 2002. © 2002 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

INTRODUCTION

Words contain many features that contribute to their meaning. Most work investigates the role of sensory‐motor feature knowledge in the neural representation of meaning for specific categories of nouns. In the present study, we compare categories of verbs to evaluate category‐specific effects in another major lexical category.

Philosophers and linguists have emphasized several features that contribute to knowledge of verb meaning. Verbs of MOTION (we use capitals to indicate a concept) such as “fall” represent observable events [Jackendoff, 1983 ; Miller and Johnson‐Laird, 1976 ]. These events typically involve planning for actions that depend on implementation by the motor system. Moreover, these events occur in a dynamic spatial framework as they unfold over time. Verbs of MOTION thus have a rich and elaborate set of associated perceptual and motor features.

By comparison, COGNITION verbs such as “ponder” are relatively impoverished in their associated motor and perceptual features. Indeed, most theories of word meaning concerned with the distinction between concrete and abstract words note that abstract words contain fewer associated sensory‐motor features [Plaut and Shallice, 1993 ; Schwanenflugel, 1991 ; Schwanenflugel and Shoben, 1983 ], and it is this paucity that accounts for our acquisition of abstract words later in development [Gentner, 1982 ] and our access to these more slowly as adults [Kroll and Merves, 1986 ; Paivio, 1991 ], in comparison to concrete terms. Several brain‐damaged patients are described with “reversal of the concreteness effect” [Breedin et al., 1995 ; Sirigu et al., 1991 ; Warrington, 1975 ; Warrington and Shallice, 1984 ], that is, these patients are more impaired in their comprehension of concrete words. Base on these observations, abstract words do not appear to differ from concrete words simply by having fewer sensory‐motor features, because the comprehension of abstract words presumably would have suffered a proportional decline following any general degradation of sensory‐motor feature knowledge. Instead, abstract words are likely to have some non‐sensory‐motor property that plays little role in concrete words, and is crucial for the mental representation and processing of abstract words. Hints about this come from developmental psycholinguistic studies suggesting that abstract words are acquired after exposure to sentences containing propositions with these terms [Landau et al., 1992 ; Landau and Jackendoff, 1993 ]. Some investigators emphasize that an elaborated set of semantic relations is particularly characteristic of the concepts underlying abstract words [Gentner, 1981 ; Jackendoff, 1990 ; Pinker, 1989 ]. Abstract words such as verbs of COGNITION thus may depend in larger part on processing an elaborate series of propositions that contribute importantly to their meaning.

Much work investigating the neural representation of category‐specific word knowledge has focused on nouns. One prominent anatomically‐based approach to categories of noun knowledge, the “sensory‐motor” hypothesis, emphasizes that semantic feature knowledge is stored in the cortical processing stream that reflects the modality (e.g., visual or motor) most relevant to the semantic feature [Allport, 1985 ; Martin et al., 2000 ; Warrington et al., 1984 ]. Functional neuroimaging studies of healthy adults thus have observed recruitment of left lateral temporal cortex during the presentation of manufactured artifacts such as TOOLS [Cappa et al., 1998 ; Chao et al., 1999 ; Damasio et al., 1996 ; Martine et al., 1996 ; Mummery et al., 1998 ; Perani et al., 1995 ], presumably reflecting the visual motion features associated with artifact use [Bonda et al., 1996 ; de Jong et al., 1994 ; Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2000 ; Patzwahl et al., 1996 ; Puce et al., 1998 ]. Some functional neuroimaging studies of healthy adults also have noted activation of premotor and prefrontal cortical regions in association with artifacts such as TOOLS [Grabowski et al., 1998 ; Grafton et al., 1997 ; Martin et al., 1996 ; Perani et al., 1995 ], presumably reflecting the motor action planning and motor implementation involved in the use of a manufactured artifact [Decety et al., 1994 ; Jeannerod et al., 1995 ; Rizzolatti et al., 1996a , b ].

Work with abstract nouns, by comparison, is much rarer [Beauregard et al., 1997 ; Grossman et al., in press ; Kiehl et al., 1999 ]. These studies associate abstract words with recruitment of left posterolateral temporal and left prefrontal brain regions. Grossman et al. ( in press ) argued that the overlap in recruitment patterns across these abstract nouns and some concrete word categories with rich sensory‐motor features such as TOOLS calls to question strong versions of the sensory‐motor hypothesis. These brain areas may be concerned not only with the neural representation of sensory‐motor features, but also with processing propositional knowledge.

There are many differences between verbs and nouns, however, which may limit generalization across these word categories. For example, the structure of the semantic network organizing verbs appears to be less deep hierarchically than for nouns [Miller and Fellbaum, 1991 ]. Verbs may depend on different configurations of sensory‐motor features, such as less dependence on imageability, compared to nouns [Bird et al., 2000 ]. Conversely, verbs appear to be much richer in their sentence‐related features, specifying associated thematic roles and other grammatical limitations that are relatively rudimentary in nouns [Jackendoff, 1983 ; Miller and Johnson‐Laird, 1976 ; Gleitman, 1990 ]. MOTION verb knowledge typically maps onto a sentence framework that contains some combination of direct and indirect objects representing the features of an event (e.g., goal, instrument) as thematic roles in a thematic matrix, in other words, an action‐oriented representation of who is doing what to whom [Baker, 1988 ; Fisher et al., 1991 ; Grimshaw, 1987 ]. COGNITION verb knowledge, by comparison, maps onto a sentence frame that typically includes a sentence complement suitable for a proposition rather than a thematic matrix (e.g., the underlined portion of the sentence “I believe that the cow jumps over the moon ”). Differences such as these may explain in part the results of many investigations showing greater difficulty in brain‐damaged patients' verb comprehension and naming relative to comparable measures with nouns [Berndt et al., 1997 ; Daniele et al., 1994 ; Miceli et al., 1984 ; Rhee and Grossman, 2001 ; White‐Devine et al., 1996 ].

Because of noun‐verb differences such as these, we cannot easily generalize from studies of the neural representation of noun categories to the neural representation of verb meaning. Only a small number of investigations have examined categories of verb knowledge directly. The association between verb meaning and left inferior frontal cortex has featured prominently in the “verb generation” task used so often since the seminal work of Petersen et al. [ 1988 ]. Some work, however, has suggested that inferior frontal activation may have less to do with verb meaning per se than task‐related processes such as the encoding and retrieval of cross‐category word associations [Demb et al., 1995 ; Kapur et al., 1994 ; Thompson‐Schill et al., 1997 ; Tulving et al., 1994 ]. More recent observations have also noted activation of left posterolateral temporal cortices during verb generation [Fiez et al., 1996 ; Warburton et al., 1996 ]. In a clever design eliciting action knowledge of object names and object pictures, verb production was associated with left inferior frontal and left posterolateral temporal recruitment [Martin et al., 1995 ]. More recently, Italian subjects were asked to make a lexical decision about printed words [Perani et al., 1999 ]. This study demonstrated recruitment of left middle and inferior frontal gyri as well as left posterolateral temporal cortex in association with verb stimuli, although the verb category was marked with a grammatical morpheme.

These activation findings for verbs have been interpreted to support the claim that the neural representation of verb knowledge is related to the left posterior temporal and prefrontal substrate of the associated sensory‐motor features. This work, however, focuses almost exclusively on MOTION verbs that refer to observable events. The one study investigating differences in category‐specific knowledge across two verb categories failed to find a category‐specific effect [Perani et al., 1999 ]. In the present study, we re‐examine the category‐specific effect in two domains of verb knowledge. Specifically, we test the generalizeability of the “sensory‐motor” account for the neural representation of word meaning by contrasting MOTION verbs with verbs of COGNITION: we examine recruitment patterns for activation of sensory‐motor association cortices related to MOTION verbs, and assess the relationship between abstract words like COGNITION verbs with left posterolateral temporal cortex where verbal, propositional knowledge may be represented and processed.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

We studied 16 right‐handed native English‐speakers who were students at the University of Pennsylvania. This includes nine females and seven males, with a mean age of 23.4 years (range 19–28 years) and a mean education of 16.0 years (range 14–18 years). These subjects participated in an informed consent procedure approved by the IRB at the University of Pennsylvania.

We presented blocks of printed words to subjects that included citation forms of MOTION verbs and COGNITION verbs. The words were either unambiguously verbs, or if not, a word's frequency of occurrence as a verb was at least five times its frequency of occurrence as a noun, according to form class‐sensitive frequency measures [Francis and Kucera, 1982 ]. The categories of verbs were matched for mean frequency [Francis and Kucera, 1982 ] (COGNITION = 16.1; MOTION = 13.8: t(118) = 0.83; NS]. A cohort of 42 native English‐speaking undergraduates assessed the words for familiarity: The meanings of all the words were known to all students, and all but 14 of the 120 words were judged to be highly familiar by over 90% of these students (one motion verb was judged familiar by only 90% of students, five cognition verbs and two motion verbs were judged familiar by only 86% of students, and three cognition verbs were judged familiar by only 83% of students).

Subjects were asked to judge the “pleasantness” of each stimulus. We chose this single probe for all words to avoid explicit categorization of verb subcategories, where the content of the word category can influence the nature of the categorization process and thus confound the interpretation of the results. “Pleasantness” decisions of this sort have been used for over 30 years to probe “deep” or “semantic” knowledge [Warrington and Weiskrantz, 1968 ]. Each printed stimulus word was available for 3 sec followed by a 1 sec ISI, and each 10‐word block lasted 40 sec. Subjects indicated their judgment of each word as pleasant or not by depressing one of two buttons (the “pleasant” button with the right hand or the “not pleasant” button with the left), and response and latency were recorded by the computer presenting the stimuli. Words were presented continuously, and blocks were presented in a fixed random order without a break between blocks of different categories. Subjects were not informed that different categories of words were being administered. Each run included two blocks of each category of verb (four blocks of verbs in total). Each run also included six blocks of filler words (nouns), and three baseline blocks (including two blocks of pronounceable pseudoword stimuli and one block of pseudofont stimuli) interspersed among the blocks of verb stimuli. Three runs containing non‐repeated stimuli were presented in total. Before each run, longitudinal magnetization was allowed to attain equilibrium while subjects were acclimated to the MRI environment during a 20 sec period of viewing the words “Get Ready” on the screen. Images corresponding to this period were discarded. Brief rest periods were included between runs.

Our stimulus presentation system (Epson 5000 LCD projector), compatible with high magnetic fields, back‐projected the printed words onto a screen at the magnet bore. The subject viewed the screen through a system of mirrors. A portable computer (Macintosh 1400C) outside the magnet room used PsyScope presentation software [Cohen et al., 1993 ] to present stimuli and record response accuracy and latency. Subjects were familiarized with the task before entering the magnet bore, and the task was practiced by each subject.

Imaging data acquisition and statistical analysis

The experiment was carried out at 1.5 T on a GE Echospeed scanner capable of ultrafast imaging. We used a standard clinical quadrature radiofrequency head coil. Firm foam padding was used to restrict head motion. Each imaging protocol began with a 10–15 min acquisition of 5 mm thick adjacent slices for determining regional anatomy, including sagittal localizer images (TR = 500 msec, TE = 10 msec, 192 × 256 matrix), T2‐weighted axial images (FSE, TR = 2,000 msec, TE eff = 85 msec), and T1‐weighted axial images of slices used for fMRI anatomic localization (TR=600 msec, TE=14 msec, 192 × 256 matrix). Gradient echo echoplanar images were acquired for detection of alterations of blood oxygenation accompanying increased mental activity. All images were acquired with fat saturation, a rectangular FOV of 20 × 15 cm, flip angle of 90°, 5 mm slice thickness, an effective TE of 50 msec, and a 64 × 40 matrix, resulting in a voxel size of 3.75 × 3.75 × 5 mm. The echoplanar acquisitions consisted of 18 contiguous slices in a transaxial plane covering most of the brain every 2 sec. A separate acquisition lasting 2 min was needed for phase maps to correct for distortion in echoplanar images. Raw data were stored by the MRI computer on DAT tape and then processed off‐line.

Initial data processing was carried out with Interactive Data Language (Research Systems) on a Sun (Santa Clara, CA) Ultra 60 workstation. Raw image data were reconstructed using a 2D FFT with a distortion correction to reduce artifact due to magnetic field inhomogeneities. Individual subject data were then prepared for pseudosubject analysis and analyzed statistically using statistical parametric mapping (SPM) developed by Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology [Frackowiak et al., 1997 ]. The SPM99 system, operating on a MatLab platform, combines raw difference images from individual subjects into a statistical t‐map using the intersubject variance. Briefly, the images in each subject's time series were registered to the initial image in the series. The images were then aligned to a standard coordinate system [Talairach and Tournoux, 1988 ]. The data were spatially smoothed with a 12 mm Gaussian kernel to account for small variations in the location of activation across subjects, and temporal smoothing was conducted with a 2.8 sec kernel to account for small variations in the hemodynamic response function. The data were analyzed parametrically using t ‐test comparisons converted to z‐scores for each compared voxel. We assessed activation associated with verbs compared to the baseline pseudoword‐pseudofont stimuli because use of nouns as a baseline would be likely to reveal different patterns of activation depending on the specific features associated with the baseline words (we found little difference as a function of pseudoword or pseudofont baseline stimuli, and thus grouped these together as a single baseline). We also directly contrasted activation associated with the categories of verbs. We borrowed the statistical approach developed for so‐called conjunction analysis to help assess differences between closely matched conditions [Price and Friston, 1997 ]. This analysis identifies the voxels that are significantly recruited in common across pairs of contrasts, in effect reducing the search space and optimizing the degrees of freedom for closely matched pairs of stimuli. For present purposes, we examined differences between verb subcategories in the context of a statistical “mask” defined by the “verb minus baseline” contrast.

Behavioral observations

Pleasantness judgments associated with each category of knowledge and latencies for making these judgments are summarized in Table ​ TableI. I . COGNITION verbs were judged to be more pleasant than MOTION verbs [t(15)=3.87; P < 0.005]. There were, however, no differences in the latency to respond during these pleasantness judgments [t(15) = 1.50; NS]. This suggests that a distinct pattern of neural recruitment for a category cannot be attributed to a difference in the relative effort involved in judging a category of verb knowledge.

Mean (±SD) pleasantness judgments and latencies for pleasantness judgments in motion and cognition verbs

Imaging observations

Table ​ TableII II provides the spatial coordinates of the peak anatomic recruitment within each significant cluster and summarizes the anatomic location of each significant cluster according to the normalized Talairach and Tournoux [1988] brain.

Locus and extent of peak activations in brain regions during pleasantness judgments of verbs

Figure ​ Figure1 1 illustrates the contrast of all verbs minus the baseline of combined pseudowords and pseudofonts. Significant recruitment (at the P < 0.001 level, uncorrected) was seen in ventral occipital and temporal regions bilaterally (Brodmann area [BA] 17, 18, 19), with greater recruitment on the left than the right. Significant activation was also evident in left posterolateral temporal cortex (BA 22, 21, 37). Prefrontal cortex activation (BA 10, 9) was bilateral, more prominently on the left than the right, at the P < 0.003 level.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is HBM-15-124-g002.jpg

Contrast of activation associated with all verb stimuli compared to a baseline including pronounceable pseudowords and pseudofonts.

Panel A of Figure ​ Figure2 2 illustrates the contrast of MOTION verbs minus COGNITION verbs. This comparison demonstrated recruitment centered in left ventral temporal‐occipital cortex (BA 19). We also observed activation of bilateral prefrontal cortex (BA 10, 11) and caudate.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is HBM-15-124-g001.jpg

Contrasts of verb subcategories. A: MOTION verbs − COGNITION verbs. B: COGNITION verbs − MOTION verbs. The locus and distribution of peak activation for each recruited region are summarized in Table ​ TableII. II . The contrasts of the two verb subcategories were derived by borrowing the statistical procedure for conjunction analysis, with (verbs − baseline) as the conjoined contrast acting as a statistical “mask”.

Panel B of Figure ​ Figure2 2 illustrates the activation pattern associated with COGNITION verbs minus MOTION verbs. This contrast revealed significant recruitment of left posterolateral temporal cortex (BA 22, 21, 37).

We found that verbs recruit bilateral ventral temporal‐occipital cortex, bilateral prefrontal cortex, and left posterolateral temporal cortex. This distribution of activation largely overlaps with the activation pattern for verbs described in previous studies [Perani et al., 1999 ; Martin et al., 1995 ; Warburton et al., 1996 ]. At first glance, inspection of the distinct activation patterns associated with each verb subcategory appears to be consistent with the “sensory‐motor” account for the category‐specific neural representation of verb knowledge: MOTION verbs were associated with left ventral temporal‐occipital, bilateral prefrontal, and caudate recruitment. Closer inspection of our results, however, suggests that this interpretation is not so straightforward: COGNITION verbs with few sensory‐motor features are associated with activation of left posterolateral temporal cortex, even though this region has been related to visual motion. We examine below the activation pattern associated with each subcategory of verb knowledge. By way of anticipation, we conclude that sources of knowledge beyond sensory‐motor features are likely to contribute to the neural activation associated with some verb categories.

MOTION verbs

Verbs of MOTION have relatively rich sensory‐motor features. This appears to parallel the concrete nouns that are typically studied in neuroimaging assessments of category‐specific lexical comprehension. Moreover, this raises the expectation that MOTION verbs are likely to have a pattern of activation similar to that associated with concrete nouns because both word subcategories denote observable objects/events. We found that MOTION verbs recruit ventral temporal‐occipital cortex more in the left hemisphere than the right hemisphere, as well as bilateral prefrontal cortex and caudate. MOTION verbs have prominent visual‐perceptual features that may be responsible for recruitment of visual association cortex [Cappa et al., 1998 ; Chao et al., 1999 ; Damasio et al., 1996 ; Martin et al., 1996 ; Mummery et al., 1998 ; Perani et al., 1995 ], and are associated with features of action planning and execution that appear to recruit premotor and prefrontal cortices [Decety et al., 1994 ; Jeannerod et al., 1995 ; Rizzolatti et al., 1996a , b ]. From the perspective of the “sensory‐motor” hypothesis, the distribution of neural activation associated with MOTION verbs thus can be attributed to the same kind of sensory‐motor features thought to contribute to the recruitment pattern seen for manufactured artifacts such as IMPLEMENTS.

Many functional neuroimaging studies of manufactured artifacts in fact demonstrate recruitment of premotor/prefrontal and posterolateral temporal cortices [Cappa et al., 1998 ; Chao et al., 1999 ; Damasio et al., 1996 ; Martin et al., 1996 ; Mummery et al., 1998 ; Perani et al., 1995 ]. Our findings are consistent with studies reported by Chao et al. [ 1999 ] and Martin et al. [ 1996 ] that emphasize the role of ventral temporal‐occipital activation in supporting visual‐perceptual features. We did not, however, observe activation for MOTION verbs in a posterolateral temporal‐occipital distribution that has been associated with the perception of visual motion [Bonda et al., 1996 ; de Jong et al., 1994 ; Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2000 ; Patzwahl et al., 1996 ; Puce et al., 1998 ]. There is reason to believe that activation for MOTION verbs may not replicate activation associated with manufactured artifacts such as IMPLEMENTS. For example, direct comparisons of behavioral judgments of MOTION verbs, manufactured artifacts and natural kinds were assessed in three patients with relative sparing of action verb naming and three patients with relatively impaired action verb naming [Bird et al., 2000 ]. These investigators reported that the “verb‐spared” patients had more difficulty naming natural kinds than manufactured artifacts, and that their definitions of objects include fewer sensory‐motor features. The naming deficit in the “verb‐impaired” patients, however, was not associated with difficulty naming manufactured artifacts like IMPLEMENTS. According to Bird et al. [ 2000 ], verb difficulties are related in part to the reduced imageability of these verbs compared to manufactured artifacts, and thus may explain in part the absence of left posterolateral temporal recruitment during MOTION verb judgments in the present experiment. Regardless of the specific basis for these noun‐verb differences, direct contrast of MOTION verbs and IMPLEMENT nouns shows distinct activation patterns as well [Koenig et al., 2001 ]. Additional work is needed to establish the precise nature of the sensory‐motor features that contribute to concrete nouns such as IMPLEMENTS and related action verbs such as verbs of MOTION.

We also observed prefrontal and caudate recruitment in association with MOTION verbs. These brain structures are part of a frontal‐striatal‐thalamic loop that has been implicated in motor functions [Alexander et al., 1990 ]. One possible explanation for frontal‐caudate recruitment is associated with the “sensory‐motor” hypothesis. Specifically, prefrontal recruitment may be related to the knowledge of action features associated with both MOTION verbs and manufactured artifacts such as IMPLEMENTS. The activation may be relatively more anterior than that typically associated with manufactured artifacts such as IMPLEMENTS because of the relatively more complex nature of lexical knowledge associated with verbs compared to nouns [Koechlin et al., 1999 , 2000 ]. Caudate recruitment appears to be associated only with MOTION verbs but not manufactured artifacts, and caudate activation may support the actual motor features that are associated only with MOTION verbs but not with IMPLEMENTS. The right‐lateralized predominance of caudate recruitment may reflect in part the spatial properties of MOTION verbs [Jackendoff, 1983 ]. The right hemispheric involvement is consistent with a report of two patients with right frontal disease who show deficits for ACTION verbs compared to concrete nouns [Neininger and Pulvermuller, 2001 ].

The frontal‐striatal loop is also implicated more directly in cognitive processes such as the executive resources that contribute to working memory [Braver et al., 2001 ; Cohen et al., 1994 ; Dagher et al., 1999 ; Grafman et al., 1990 ; Owen et al., 1998 ; Smith and Jonides, 1999 ], and another account relates frontal‐caudate activation to the complex nature of thematic roles that are associated with verbs of MOTION. MOTION verbs are typically related to a variety of direct or indirect objects, corresponding to the potentially large array of thematic roles with which a verb can be linked. Some evidence to support this claim comes from the observation that verbs with larger numbers of optional thematic roles are more difficult to understand than verbs with fewer optional thematic roles [Shapiro et al., 1987 , 1993 ]. Consistent with this potential need to manage a complex network of optional thematic roles, several functional neuroimaging studies demonstrate recruitment in a frontal‐caudate distribution during resource‐demanding verbal measures [Cooke et al., 2000 ; Poldrack et al., 1999 ; Rypma et al., 1999 ]. This is less likely to account for frontal‐caudate recruitment only during presentation of MOTION verbs, however, given the apparently equal levels of difficulty for MOTION verbs and COGNITION verbs suggested by the equivalent latency measures.

Another less likely possibility is that medial prefrontal recruitment is related to the nature of the probe we used to investigate verb meaning. “Pleasantness” was used recently to probe self‐referential mental activity during judgments of pleasant and unpleasant pictures [Gusnard et al., 2001 ]. Medial prefrontal activity was associated with “pleasantness” ratings. There are several reasons to doubt this account. For one, direct contrast of MOTION verbs and COGNITION verbs should minimize this component of our task because “pleasantness” is probed for both categories of verbs. Even if “pleasantness” is associated unequally with these two verb categories, Gusnard et al. [ 2001 ] would predict medial prefrontal activation for the more affectively charged COGNITION verbs (i.e., “pleasantness” judgments that deviate more from 0.5), when in fact we find prefrontal activity associated with MOTION verbs rather than COGNITION verbs. Finally, we do not see prefrontal activation for all categories of nouns we probed with the “pleasantness” task [Grossman et al., in press ]. In like manner, the association of prefrontal activation with novel, attention‐demanding tasks and materials that require the integration of cognitive and emotional attributes [Drevets and Raichle, 1998 ; Shulman et al., 1997 ; Simpson et al., 2001 ] is unlikely to explain the preferential association of this region with only one verb category. Additional work is needed to determine the precise basis for frontal‐caudate recruitment during lexical processing.

COGNITION verbs

Although some evidence consistent with the “sensory‐motor” hypothesis may be derived from the pattern of neural activation associated with MOTION verbs, inspection of brain activation during presentation of COGNITION verbs is less supportive of a strong version of the “sensory‐motor” hypothesis that attempts to account for the meaning of all verbs. Verbs of COGNITION are associated with recruitment of left posterolateral temporal cortex. Other studies investigating the neural basis for word comprehension that use MOTION verbs as stimuli also recruit this posterolateral temporal region [Fiez et al., 1996 ; Martin et al., 1995 ; Perani et al., 1999 ; Warburton et al., 1996 ]. This is attributed to visual‐perceptual features such as motion that are a property of the stimulus word and that recruit visual association cortex in a posterolateral temporal‐occipital distribution [Bonda et al., 1996 ; de Jong et al., 1994 ; Kourtzi et al., 2000 ; Patzwahl et al., 1996 ; Puce et al., 1998 ]. The pattern of activation associated with these findings is interpreted to support the “sensory‐motor” approach to the neural representation of verb meaning. One problem with this line of reasoning is that these observations are based primarily on verbs associated with observable events, whereas verbs of COGNITION such as “think” and “speculate” are quite impoverished in their visual‐perceptual and motor‐action features.

It is possible to couch differences in the distribution of neural recruitment for MOTION verbs and COGNITION verbs in the context of the “dual coding” hypothesis [Kounios and Holcomb, 2000 ; Paivio, 1971 , 1978 , 1991 ]. According to this approach, concrete words can be encoded in a visual, image‐based manner and as verbal‐propositional knowledge, whereas abstract words can be encoded only in a verbal‐propositional manner. Analogously, the sensory‐motor features of MOTION verbs may allow these words to be encoded by the visual, image‐based system in visual association cortex and as verbal‐propositional knowledge represented in peri‐Sylvian regions of the left hemisphere, whereas encoding of COGNITION verbs may be largely restricted to verbal‐propositional knowledge. One problem with this proposal is that there is no brain area encoding verbal‐propositional knowledge that is recruited for both categories of verbs. Moreover, this kind of approach would have difficulty explaining “reversal of the concreteness effect” seen in some patients [Breedin et al., 1995 ; Sirigu et al., 1991 ; Warrington, 1975 , 1984 ]. Based on observations such as these, COGNITION verbs appear to possess features that are relatively unique to abstract words and that are not explained by the general degradation of sensory‐motor feature knowledge. The feature distinct to abstract words such as COGNITION verbs may be the elaborated set of semantic relations characteristic of the concepts underlying abstract words [Gentner, 1981 ; Jackendoff, 1990 ; Pinker, 1989 ], and it is the processing of the complex interaction of propositional features that may be responsible for left posterolateral temporal activation. This would be consistent with the neuroanatomic connectivity pattern of posterolateral temporal cortex, a supramodal region that has reciprocal connections with multiple association cortices [Mesulam, 1985 ]. ABSTRACT nouns recruit left posterolateral temporal cortex and prefrontal cortex compared to pronounceable pseudowords and compared to concrete nouns such as ANIMALS [Beauregard et al., 1997 ; Grossman et al., in press ; Perani et al., 1999 ]. We speculated that the anatomic connectivity pattern of this posterior supramodal association region is ideally suited to the collection and organization of the propositional knowledge associated with abstract word meaning that is widely distributed throughout association cortices [Grossman et al., in press ]. This account may help explain left posterolateral temporal recruitment for COGNITION verbs as well.

Acknowledgements

We express our appreciation to Dr. John Kounios and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

  • Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR (1990): Basal ganglia‐thalamocortical circuits: Parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, prefrontal and limbic functions . Prog Brain Res 85 : 119–146. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Allport DA (1985): Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphasia In: Newman SK, Epstein R, editors. Current perspectives in dysphasia . Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone; p 207–224. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baker M (1988): Incorporation: a theory of grammatical function changing . Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beauregard M, Chertkow H, Bub D, Murtha S, Dixon R, Evans A (1997): The neural substrate for concrete, abstract, and emotional word lexica: a positron emission tomography study . J Cogn Neurosci 9 : 441‐461. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berndt RS, Mitchum CC, Haendiges AN, Sandson J (1997): Verb retrieval in aphasia: 1. Characterizing single word impairments . Brain Lang 56 : 68–106. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bird H, Howard D, Franklin S (2000): Why is a verb like an inanimate object? Grammatical category and semantic category deficits . Brain Lang 72 : 246–309. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bonda E, Petrides M, Ostry D, Evans A (1996): Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion . J Neurosci 16 : 3737–3744. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braver TS, Barch DM, Kelley WM, Buckner RL, Cohen NJ, Miezin FM, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Petersen SE (2001): Direct comparison of prefrontal cortex regions engaged by working and long‐term memory tasks . Neuroimage 14 : 48–59. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Breedin SD, Saffran EM, Coslett HB (1995): Reversal of a concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia . Cogn Neuropsychol 11 : 617–660. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cappa SF, Perani D, Schnur T, Tettamanti M, Fazio F (1998): The effects of semantic category and knowledge type on lexical‐semantic access: a PET study . Neuroimage 8 : 350–359. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chao LL, Haxby J, Martin A (1999): Attribute‐based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects . Nat Neurosci 2 : 913–919. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen JD, Forman SD, Braver TS, Casey BJ, Servan‐Schreiber D, Noll DC (1994): Activation of prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI . Hum Brain Mapp 1 : 293–304. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen JD, MacWhinney B, Flatt MR, Provost J (1993): PsyScope: a new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments . Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput 25 : 101–113. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooke A, DeVita C, Gethers C, Alsop D, Gee JC, Detre J, Koenig P, Glosser G, Stern MB, Hurtig HI, Grossman M (2000): Information processing speed during functional neuroimaging of sentence comprehension . J Cogn Neurosci 12 : S43. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dagher A, Owen AM, Boecker H, Brooks DJ (1999): Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task . Brain 122 : 1973–1987. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Damasio H, Grabowski TJ, Tranel D, Hichwa RD, Damasio AR (1996): A neural basis for lexical retrieval . Nature 380 : 499–505. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Daniele A, Giustolisi L, Silveri MC, Colosimo C, Gainotti G (1994): Evidence for a possible neuroanatomical basis for the lexical processing of nouns and verbs . Neuropsychologia 32 : 1325–1341. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Jong BM, Shipp S, Skidmore B, Frackowiak RSJ, Zeki S (1994): The visual activity related to motion in depth . Brain 117 : 1039–1052. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Decety JD, Perani D, Jeannerod M, Bettinardi V, Tadary B, Woods RP, Mazziotta JC, Fazio F (1994): Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography . Nature 371 : 600–602. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Demb JB, Desmond JE, Wagner AD, Vaidya CJ, Glover GH, Gabrieli JDE (1995): Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity . J Neurosci 15 : 5870–5878. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drevets WC, Raichle M (1998): Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood flow during emotional versus cognitive processes: implications for interactions between emotion and cognition . Cognit Emot 12 : 353–385. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiez JA, Raichle ME, Balota DA, Tallal P, Petersen SE (1996): PET activation of posterior temporal regions during auditory word presentation and verb generation . Cereb Cortex 6 : 1–10. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fisher C, Gleitman H, Gleitman LR (1991): On the semantic content of subcategorization frames . Cognit Psychol 23 : 331–392. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Frackowiak RSJ, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Mazziotta JC (1997): Human brain function . San Diego: Academic Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Francis WN, Kucera H (1982): The frequency analysis of English usage . First edition Boston: Houghton‐Mifflin Co. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gentner D (1981): Some interesting differences between verbs and nouns . Cognit Brain Theory 4 : 161–178. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gentner D (1982): Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning In: Kuczaj S, editor. Language development, vol. 2 Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc; p 301–334. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gleitman LR (1990): The structural sources for verb meanings . Lang Acquisition 1 : 3–55. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grabowski TJ, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1998): Premotor and prefrontal correlates of category‐related lexical retrieval . Neuroimage 7 : 232–243. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grafman J, Jonas B, Salazar A (1990): Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance based on location and size of neuroanatomical lesion in Vietnam veterans with penetrating head injuries . Percept Mot Skills 71 : 1120–1122. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grafton ST, Padiga L, Arbib MA, Rizzolatti G (1997): Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools . Neuroimage 6 : 231–236. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grimshaw J (1987): Unaccusatives: an overview In: McDonough J, Plunkett B, editors. Proceedings of NELS 17, Vol. 1 Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grossman M, Koenig P, DeVita C, Glosser G, Alsop D, Detre J, Gee JC (in press): The neural basis for category‐specific knowledge: an fMRI study . Neuroimage . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME (2001): Medial prefrontal cortex and self‐referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function . Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98 : 4259–4264. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jackendoff R (1983): Semantics and cognition . Cambridge: MIT Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jackendoff R (1990): Semantic structures . First edition Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jeannerod M, Arbib MA, Rizzolatti G, Sakata H (1995): Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation . Trends Neurosci 18 : 314–320. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kapur S, Rose R, Little PF, Zipursky RB, Brown GM, Stuss D, Houle S, Tulving E (1994): The role of the left prefrontal cortex in verbal processing: semantic processing or willed action? Neuroreport 5 : 2193–2196. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kiehl KA, Liddle PF, Smith AM, Mendrek A, Forster BB, Hare RD (1999): Neural pathways involved in the processing of concrete and abstract words . Hum Brain Mapp 7 : 225–233. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J (1999): The role of anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition . Nature 399 : 148–151. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koechlin E, Corrado G, Pietrini P, Grafman J (2000): Dissociating the role of the medial and lateral anterior prefrontal cortex in human planning . Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97 : 7651–7656. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koenig P, DeVita C, Moore P, Glosser G, Detre J, Alsop D, Gee JC, Grossman M (submitted): Implements and actions: an fMRI study of word meaning for semantically‐related categories .
  • Kounios J, Holcomb PJ (2000): Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual‐coding theory . J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 20 : 804–823. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N (2000): Activation in human MT/MST by static images with implied motion . J Cogn Neurosci 12 : 48–55. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kroll JF, Merves JS (1986): Lexical access for concrete and abstract words . J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 12 : 92–107. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Landau B, Jackendoff R (1993): “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition . Behav Brain Sci 16 : 217–265. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Landau B, Smith LB, Jones S (1992): Syntactic context and the shape bias in children's and adults' early lexical learning . J Mem Lang 31 : 807–825. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin A, Haxby JV, Lalonde FM, Wiggs CL, Ungerleider LG (1995): Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action . Science 270 : 102–105. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin A, Ungerleider L, Haxby JV (2000): Category specificity and the brain: the sensory/motor model of semantic representations of objects In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The new cognitive neurosciences . Cambridge: MIT Press; p 1023–1036. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin A, Wiggs CL, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1996): Neural correlates of category‐specific knowledge . Nature 379 : 649–652. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mesulam M‐M (1985): Patterns in behavioral neuroanatomy: association areas, the limbic system, and hemispheric specialization In: Mesulam M.‐M, editor. Principles of behavioral neurology . Philadelphia: FA Davis Company; p 1–70. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miceli G, Silveri MC, Villa G, Caramazza A (1984): On the basis for the agrammatic's difficulty in producing main verbs . Cortex 20 : 207–220. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller GA, Fellbaum C (1991): Semantic networks of English . Cognition 41 : 197–229. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller GA, Johnson‐Laird PN (1976): Language and perception . First edition Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mummery CJ, Patterson K, Hodges JR, Price CJ (1998): Functional neuroanatomy of the semantic system: divisible by what? J Cogn Neurosci 10 : 766–777. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Neininger B, Pulvermuller F (2001): The right hemisphere's role in action word processing: a double case study . Neurocase 7 : 303–317. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Owen AM, Doyon J, Dagher A, Sadikot A, Evans AC (1998): Abnormal basal ganglia outflow in Parkinson's disease identified with PET: implications for higher cortical functions . Brain 121 : 949–965. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paivio A (1971): Imagery and verbal processes . New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paivio A (1978): Imagery, language and semantic memory . Int J Psycholinguistics 5 : 31–47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paivio A (1991): Dual coding theory: retrospect and current status . Can J Psychol 45 : 255–287. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Patzwahl DR, Elbert T, Zanker JM, Altenmuller EO (1996): The cortical representation of object motion in man is interindividually variable . Neuroreport 7 : 469–472. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perani D, Cappa S, Schnur T, Tettamanti M, Collina S, Rosa MM, Fazio F (1999): The neural correlates of verb and noun processing: a PET study . Brain 122 : 2337–2344. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perani D, Cappa SF, Bettinardi V, Bressi S, Gorno‐Tempini M, Matarrese M, Fazio F (1995): Different neural systems for the recognition of animals and man‐made tools . Neuroreport 6 : 1637–1641. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Petersen SE, Fox PT, Posner MI, Mintun M, Raichle ME (1988): Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single word processing . Nature 331 : 585–589. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pinker S (1989): Learnability and cognition: the acquisition of argument structure . Cambridge: MIT Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Plaut DC, Shallice T (1993): Deep dyslexia: a case study of connectionist neuropsychology . Cogn Neuropsychol 10 : 377–500. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Poldrack RA, Prabhakaran V, Seger CA, Gabrieli JDE (1999): Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill . Neuropsychology 13 : 564–574. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Price CJ, Friston K (1997): Cognitive conjunction: a new approach to brain activation experiments . Neuroimage 5 : 261–270. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Puce A, Allison T, Bentin S, Gore JC, McCarthy G (1998): Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movement . J Neurosci 18 : 2188–2199. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rhee J, Grossman M (2001): Verb comprehension in frontotemporal degeneration: the role of grammatical, semantic and executive components . Neurocase 7 : 173–184. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Matelli M, Bettinardi V, Paulesu E, Perani D, Fazio F (1996a): Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation vs. execution . Exp Brain Res 111 : 246–252. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Mattelli M, Bettinardi V, Paulesu E, Perani D (1996b): Localization of cortical areas responsive to the observation of hand grasping movements in humans: a PET study . Exp Brain Res 112 : 103‐111. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JDE (1999): Load‐dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory . Neuroimage 9 : 216–226. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwanenflugel PJ (1991): Why are abstract concepts hard to understand? In: Schwanenflugel PJ, editor. The psychology of word meanings . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwanenflugel PJ, Shoben EJ (1983): Differential context effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials . J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 9 : 82–102. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shapiro LP, Gordon B, Hack N, Killackey J (1993): Verb‐argument structure processing in complex sentences in Broca and Wernicke aphasia . Brain Lang 45 : 423–447. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shapiro LP, Zurif EB, Grimshaw J (1987): Sentence processing and the mental representation of verbs . Cognition 27 : 219–246. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE (1997): Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex . J Cogn Neurosci 9 : 648–663. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Simpson JR Jr, Snyder AZ, Gusnard DA, Raichle ME (2001): Emotion‐induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex: I. During cognitive task performance . Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98 : 683–687. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sirigu A, Duhamel J‐R, Poncet M (1991): The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition . Brain 114 : 2555–2573. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith EE, Jonides J (1999): Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes . Science 283 : 1657–1661. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988): Co‐planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain . First edition New York: Thieme Medical Publishing Company. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson‐Schill SL, D'Esposito M, Aguirre G, Farah MJ (1997): Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation . Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94 : 14792–14797. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tulving E, Kapur S, Craik FIM, Moscovitch M, Houle S (1994): Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron emission tomography findings . Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91 : 2016–2020. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warburton E, Wise RJS, Price CJ, Weiller C, Hadar U, Ramsay S, Frackowiak RSJ (1996): Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects: studies with PET . Brain 119 : 159–179. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warrington EK (1975): The selective impairment of semantic memory . Q J Exp Psychol 27 : 635–657. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warrington EK, Shallice T (1984): Category‐specific semantic impairments . Brain 107 : 829–854. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warrington EK, Weiskrantz L (1968): New method of testing long‐term retention with special reference to amnesic patients . Nature 217 : 972–974. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • White‐Devine T, Robinson KM, Onishi K, Seidl A, Biassou N, D'Esposito M, Grossman M (1996): Verb confrontation naming and word‐picture matching in Alzheimer disease . Neuropsychology 10 : 495–503. [ Google Scholar ]

Go to the homepage

Synonyms of 'representation' in American English

  • representation

Synonyms of 'representation' in British English

Additional synonyms, video: pronunciation of representation.

Youtube video

Browse alphabetically representation

  • represent someone as something or someone
  • represent yourself as something or someone
  • representative
  • All ENGLISH synonyms that begin with 'R'

Quick word challenge

Quiz Review

Score: 0 / 5

Tile

Wordle Helper

Tile

Scrabble Tools

Mental Representation of Verb Meaning: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence

Ieee account.

  • Change Username/Password
  • Update Address

Purchase Details

  • Payment Options
  • Order History
  • View Purchased Documents

Profile Information

  • Communications Preferences
  • Profession and Education
  • Technical Interests
  • US & Canada: +1 800 678 4333
  • Worldwide: +1 732 981 0060
  • Contact & Support
  • About IEEE Xplore
  • Accessibility
  • Terms of Use
  • Nondiscrimination Policy
  • Privacy & Opting Out of Cookies

A not-for-profit organization, IEEE is the world's largest technical professional organization dedicated to advancing technology for the benefit of humanity. © Copyright 2024 IEEE - All rights reserved. Use of this web site signifies your agreement to the terms and conditions.

Meaning Representation

  • First Online: 15 November 2023

Cite this chapter

Book cover

  • Raymond S. T. Lee 2  

1132 Accesses

Before the study of semantic analysis, this chapter explores meaning representation, a vital component in NLP before the discussion of semantic and pragmatic analysis. It studies four major meaning representation techniques which include: first-order predicate calculus (FOPC), semantic net, conceptual dependency diagram (CDD), and frame-based representation. After that it explores canonical form and introduces Fillmore’s theory of universal cases followed by predicate logic and inference work using FOPC with live examples.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Bender, E. M. (2013) Linguistic Fundamentals for Natural Language Processing: 100 Essentials from Morphology and Syntax (Synthesis Lectures on Human Language Technologies). Morgan & Claypool Publishers

Book   Google Scholar  

Bender, E. M. and Lascarides, A. (2019) Linguistic Fundamentals for Natural Language Processing II: 100 Essentials from Semantics and Pragmatics (Synthesis Lectures on Human Language Technologies). Springer.

Google Scholar  

Best, W., Bryan, K. and Maxim, J. (2000) Semantic Processing: Theory and Practice. Wiley.

Blackburn, P and Bos, J. (2005) Representation and Inference for Natural Language: A First Course in Computational Semantics (Studies in Computational Linguistics). Center for the Study of Language and Information.

Bunt, H. (2013) Computing Meaning: Volume 4 (Text, Speech and Language Technology Book 47). Springer.

Butler, A. (2015) Linguistic Expressions and Semantic Processing: A Practical Approach. Springer.

Book   MATH   Google Scholar  

Cui, Y., Huang, C., Lee, Raymond (2020). AI Tutor: A Computer Science Domain Knowledge Graph-Based QA System on JADE platform. World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology, Open Science Index 168, International Journal of Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering, 14(12), 543 - 553.

Dijkstra, E. W. and Scholten, C. S. (1989) Predicate Calculus and Program Semantics (Monographs in Computer Science). Springer. Advanced Reasoning Forum.

Doyle, A. C. (2019) The Adventures of Sherlock Holmes (AmazonClassics Edition). AmazonClassics.

Epstein, R. (2012) Predicate Logic. Advanced Reasoning Forum.

Fillmore, C. J. (1968) The Case for Case. In Bach and Harms (Ed.): Universals in Linguistic Theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1-88.

Fillmore, C. J. (2020) Form and Meaning in Language, Volume III: Papers on Linguistic Theory and Constructions (Volume 3). Center for the Study of Language and Information.

Goddard, C. (1998) Semantic Analysis: A Practical Introduction (Oxford Textbooks in Linguistics). Oxford University Press.

Goldrei, D. (2005) Propositional and Predicate Calculus: A Model of Argument. Springer.

MATH   Google Scholar  

Jackson, P. C. (2019) Toward Human-Level Artificial Intelligence: Representation and Computation of Meaning in Natural Language (Dover Books on Mathematics). Dover Publications.

Mazarweh, S. (2010) Fillmore Case Grammar: Introduction to the Theory. GRIN Verlag.

Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. In P. Winston, Ed., The Psychology of Computer Vision. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 211-277.

Parry, W. T. and Hacker, E. A. (1991) Aristotelian logic. Suny Press.

Potts, T. C. (1994) Structures and Categories for the Representation of Meaning. Cambridge University Press.

Schank, R. C. (1972). Conceptual dependency: A theory of natural language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 552–631.

Article   Google Scholar  

Sowa, J. (1991) Principles of Semantic Networks: Explorations in the Representation of Knowledge (Morgan Kaufmann Series in Representation and Reasoning). Morgan Kaufmann Publication.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

United International College, Beijing Normal University-Hong Kong Baptist University, Zhuhai, China

Raymond S. T. Lee

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2024 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Lee, R.S.T. (2024). Meaning Representation. In: Natural Language Processing. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-1999-4_5

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-1999-4_5

Published : 15 November 2023

Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore

Print ISBN : 978-981-99-1998-7

Online ISBN : 978-981-99-1999-4

eBook Packages : Computer Science Computer Science (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. Types of Verbs with Examples in English

    representation meaning verb

  2. Verbs List with Meaning

    representation meaning verb

  3. Types of Verbs (Examples & List)

    representation meaning verb

  4. Verbs: 3 Types of Verbs with Definition and Useful Examples

    representation meaning verb

  5. Conjugation Represent 🔸 Verb in all tenses and forms

    representation meaning verb

  6. 200 Important Regular Verbs: Definition and Regular Verbs List

    representation meaning verb

VIDEO

  1. Representation

  2. What is a Verb?

  3. Delineate Unraveling the Meaning

  4. Pictorial representation

  5. What is the meaning of the word MANTISSA?

  6. What is the meaning of the word INSTANTIATION?

COMMENTS

  1. Representation Definition & Meaning

    representation: [noun] one that represents: such as. an artistic likeness or image. a statement or account made to influence opinion or action. an incidental or collateral statement of fact on the faith of which a contract is entered into. a dramatic production or performance. a usually formal statement made against something or to effect a ...

  2. Represent Definition & Meaning

    The meaning of REPRESENT is to bring clearly before the mind : present. How to use represent in a sentence. ... Verb (1) He represented his company at the meeting. ... to provide legal representation to as a lawyer. c: to act as the representative of in a class action. 2. a

  3. represent verb

    [often passive] represent somebody/something to be a member of a group of people and act or speak for them at an event, a meeting, etc. The competition attracted over 500 contestants representing eight different countries. Local businesses are well represented on the committee (= there are a lot of people from them on the committee).; The opening speech was by Bob Alan representing Amnesty ...

  4. REPRESENTATION Definition & Meaning

    Representation definition: the act of representing.. See examples of REPRESENTATION used in a sentence.

  5. REPRESENT

    REPRESENT definition: 1. to speak, act, or be present officially for another person or people: 2. to be the Member of…. Learn more.

  6. REPRESENTATION definition

    REPRESENTATION meaning: 1. a person or organization that speaks, acts, or is present officially for someone else: 2. the…. Learn more.

  7. representation

    There has been a decline in union representation in the auto industry. → proportional representation 2 [ countable] a painting, sign, description etc that shows something representation of The clock in the painting is a symbolic representation of the passage of time. 3 [ uncountable] the act of representing someone or something representation ...

  8. REPRESENT definition in American English

    represent in American English. (ˌreprɪˈzent) transitive verb. 1. to serve to express, designate, stand for, or denote, as a word, symbol, or the like does; symbolize. In this painting the cat represents evil and the bird, good. 2. to express or designate by some term, character, symbol, or the like.

  9. representation noun

    representation from all parties; representation for employees; representation by a lawyer; direct representation in Parliament; Whether guilty or innocent, we are still entitled to legal representation. They had a strong representation in government. The task force had broad representation with members drawn from different departments.

  10. REPRESENTATION definition in American English

    representation in American English. (ˌrɛprɪzɛnˈteɪʃən ) noun. 1. a representing or being represented (in various senses); specif., the fact of representing or being represented in a legislative assembly. 2. legislative representatives, collectively. 3. a likeness, image, picture, etc.

  11. representation noun

    Definition of representation noun in Oxford Advanced American Dictionary. Meaning, pronunciation, picture, example sentences, grammar, usage notes, synonyms and more.

  12. Represent Definition & Meaning

    represent /ˌrɛprɪ ˈ zɛnt/ verb. represents; represented; representing. Britannica Dictionary definition of REPRESENT. 1. [+ object] a : to act or speak officially for (someone or something) He represented his company at the meeting. She hired an agent to represent her in the contract negotiations. b : to have a government position in which ...

  13. Represent

    Use the word represent when something stands in for something else — whether it's your congressman representing your interests in Washington or the thimble that represents you as it makes its way around the Monopoly board.

  14. REPRESENTATION definition and meaning

    10 meanings: 1. the act or an instance of representing or the state of being represented 2. anything that represents, such as a.... Click for more definitions.

  15. (PDF) Cognitive Science Perspectives on Verb Representation and

    Cognitive science perspectives on verb representation and processing. pp.3-40. Verbs are said to play a central role in the lexicalization of events and states—thus they are crucial for ...

  16. PDF Lexical Semantics of Verbs I: Introduction and Role-Centered Approaches

    Role-Centered Approaches to Lexical Semantic Representation Part I: Introduction The Big Questions: How do we discover meaning of verbs? → The major focus here. How do we represent meaning of verbs? → The secondary focus. 1 The Foundational Assumption: Verb Meaning Provides a Key to Verb Behavior Why is a theory of verb meaning important?

  17. Mental Representation of Verb Meaning: Behavioral and

    Abstract. Previous psycholinguistic research has debated the nature of the mental representation of verbs and the access of relevant verb information in sentence processing. In this study, we used behavioral and electrophysiological methods to examine the representation of verbs in and out of sentence contexts. In five experiments, word naming and event-related potential (ERP) components were ...

  18. Mental Representation of Verb Meaning: Behavioral and

    Verb Meaning Representation Based on Structured Semantic Components. Chinese Lexical Semantics . Abstract. Meaning representation is a key task in computational linguistics and natural language processing. Many current meaning representation models fail to represent the accurate meanings of some sentences because of the flexibility of natural ...

  19. Neural representation of verb meaning: An fMRI study

    The association between verb meaning and left inferior frontal cortex has featured prominently in the "verb generation" task used so often since the seminal work of Petersen et al. ... of activation associated with these findings is interpreted to support the "sensory‐motor" approach to the neural representation of verb meaning. One ...

  20. Synonyms of REPRESENTATION

    the representation of women in film and literature. Synonyms. portrayal. a near-monochrome portrayal of a wood infused with silvery light . depiction. ... If you say that someone demands their pound of flesh , you mean that they insist on getting something they are entitled to, even though it may cause distress to the person it is demanded from.

  21. Mental Representation of Verb Meaning: Behavioral and

    The results suggest that the semantic features of the verb's arguments are an integral part of the mental representation of verbs, and such information of the verbs is accessed and used on-line during sentence processing. Previous psycholinguistic research has debated the nature of the mental representation of verbs and the access of relevant verb information in sentence processing.

  22. Mental Representation of Verb Meaning: Behavioral and

    Previous psycholinguistic research has debated the nature of the mental representation of verbs and the access of relevant verb information in sentence processing. In this study, we used behavioral and electrophysiological methods to examine the representation of verbs in and out of sentence contexts. In five experiments, word naming and event-related potential (ERP) components were used to ...

  23. Meaning Representation

    Meaning is the message to convey by words, phrases, and sentences/utterances with context in linguistics. It is also called lexical or semantic meanings. Prof. W Tecumseh Fitch described semantics meaning in The Evolution of Language (Fitch 2010) as a branch of language study that consistently related with philosophy.