Attention! Your ePaper is waiting for publication!

By publishing your document, the content will be optimally indexed by Google via AI and sorted into the right category for over 500 million ePaper readers on YUMPU.

This will ensure high visibility and many readers!

illustration

Your ePaper is now published and live on YUMPU!

You can find your publication here:

Share your interactive ePaper on all platforms and on your website with our embed function

illustration

Critical Thinking Disposition Self- Rating Form. - Pearson Learning ...

  • disposition
  • pearsoncustom.com

pearsoncustom.com

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Think <strong>Critical</strong>ly, by Peter Facione and Carol Ann Gittens. Published by Prentice Hall. Copyright © 2013 by <strong>Pearson</strong> Education, Inc.<br />

000200010271662400

Read the Chapter on mythinkinglab.com<br />

Listen to the Chapter Audio on mythinkinglab.com<br />

After training<br />

for every conceivable contingency, the unexpected<br />

happened. Initially, the challenge was simply to figure<br />

out what the problem was. If it could be correctly<br />

identified, then there might be some slim chance of<br />

survival. If not, the outcome could be tragic.<br />

Watch the Video on mythinkinglab.com<br />

As you watch the<br />

video clip at www.mythinkinglab.com, keep in mind<br />

that you are seeing a dramatic reenactment. The actors,<br />

music, camera angles, staging, props, and lighting<br />

all contribute to our overall experience. That said,<br />

this portrayal of individual and group problem solving<br />

is highly consistent with the research on human<br />

cognition and decision making. i The clip dramatizes<br />

a group of people engaged in thinking critically together<br />

about one thing: What could the problem be<br />

Their approach is to apply their reasoning skills to the<br />

best of their ability. But, more than only their thinking<br />

skills, their mental habits of being analytical, focused,<br />

and systematic enabled them to apply those skills<br />

well during the moment of crisis. We suggest that you<br />

watch the brief video prior to reading the summary<br />

analysis of Apollo 13.<br />

CHAPTER<br />

Skilled and<br />

Eager to<br />

Think<br />

QHOW can I cultivate positive<br />

critical thinking habits of mind<br />

WHAT questions can I ask to<br />

engage my critical thinking<br />

skills<br />

WHAT are induction and<br />

deduction, and how do they<br />

differ<br />

HOW can I best use this book<br />

to develop my critical thinking<br />

02<br />

19<br />

000200010271662400<br />

Chapter 02 20<br />

Positive <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong><br />

Habits of Mind<br />

The Apollo 13 sequence opens with the staff at Mission Control<br />

in Houston and the three-person crew of Apollo 13 well<br />

into the boredom of routine housekeeping. Suddenly, the crew<br />

of Apollo 13 hears a loud banging noise and their small, fragile<br />

craft starts gyrating wildly. The startled look on Tom Hanks’s<br />

face in the video reenactment is priceless. A full fifteen seconds<br />

elapses before he speaks. During that time his critical<br />

thinking is in overdrive. He is trying to interpret what has just<br />

happened. His mind has to make sense of the entirely unexpected<br />

and unfamiliar experience. He neither dismisses nor<br />

ignores the new information that presents itself. His attention<br />

moves between checking the craft’s instrument panel and<br />

attending to the sounds and motions of the spacecraft itself.<br />

He focuses his mind, forms a cautious but accurate interpretation,<br />

and with the disciplined self-control we expect of a welltrained<br />

professional, he informs Mission Control in Houston,<br />

Texas that they most definitely have a problem! ii<br />

At first, the astronauts in the spacecraft and the technicians<br />

at Mission Control call out information from their desk monitors<br />

and the spacecraft’s instrument displays. They crave information<br />

from all sources. They know they must share what<br />

they are learning with each other as quickly as they can in the<br />

hope that someone will be able to make sense out of things.<br />

They do not yet know which piece of information may be the<br />

clue to their life-or-death problem, but they have the discipline<br />

of mind to want to know everything that might be relevant.<br />

And they have the confidence in their collective critical thinking<br />

skills to believe that this approach offers their best hope to<br />

identify the true problem.<br />

One member of the ground crew calls out, that O 2<br />

Tank<br />

Two is not showing any readings. That vital bit of information<br />

swooshes by almost unnoticed in the torrents of data. Soon<br />

a number of people begin proposing explanations: Perhaps<br />

the spacecraft had been struck by a meteor. Perhaps its radio<br />

antenna is broken. Perhaps the issue is instrumentation, rather<br />

than something more serious, like a loss of power.<br />

The vital critical thinking skill of <strong>Self</strong>-Regulation is personified<br />

in the movie by the character played by Ed Harris. His job<br />

is to monitor everything that is going on and to correct the process<br />

if he judges that it is getting off track. Harris’s character<br />

makes the claim that the problem cannot simply be instrumentation.<br />

The reason for that claim is clear and reasonable.<br />

The astronauts are reporting hearing loud bangs and feeling<br />

their spacecraft jolt and shimmy. The unspoken assumption,<br />

one every pilot and technician understands in this context<br />

is that these physical manifestations—the noises and the<br />

shaking—would not be occurring if the problem were instrumentation.<br />

The conclusion Harris’s character expressed has<br />

the effect of directing everyone’s energy and attention toward<br />

one set of possibilities, those that would be considered real<br />

problems rather than toward the other set of possibilities. Had<br />

he categorized the problem as instrumentation, then everyone’s<br />

efforts would have been directed toward checking and<br />

verifying that the gauges and computers were functioning<br />

properly.<br />

There is a very important lesson for good critical thinking in<br />

what we see Ed Harris doing. Judging correctly what kind of<br />

problem we are facing is essential. If we are mistaken about<br />

what the problem is, we are likely to consume time, energy,<br />

and resources exploring the wrong kinds of solutions. By the<br />

time we figure out that we took the wrong road, the situation<br />

could have become much worse than when we started.<br />

The Apollo 13 situation is a perfect example. In real life, had<br />

the people at Mission Control in Houston classified the problem<br />

as instrumentation, they would have used up what little<br />

oxygen there was left aboard the spacecraft while the ground<br />

crew spent time validating their instrument readouts.<br />

Back on the spacecraft, Tom Hanks, who personifies the<br />

critical thinking skills of interpretation and inference, is struggling<br />

to regain navigational control. He articulates the inference<br />

by saying that had they been hit by a meteor, they would all be<br />

dead already. A few moments later he glances out the spacecraft’s<br />

side window. Something in the rearview mirror catches<br />

his attention. Again, his inquisitive mind will not ignore what<br />

he’s seeing. A few seconds pass as he tries to interpret what it<br />

might be. He offers his first observation that the craft is venting<br />

something into space. The mental focus and stress of the entire<br />

Houston ground crew are etched on their faces. Their expressions<br />

reveal the question in their minds: What could he possibly<br />

be seeing Seconds pass with agonizing slowness. Using<br />

his interpretive skills, Tom Hanks categorizes with caution and<br />

then, adding greater precision, he infers that the venting must<br />

be some kind of a gas. He pauses to try to figure out what the<br />

gas might be and realizes that it must surely be the oxygen.<br />

Kevin Bacon looks immediately to the oxygen tank gauge on<br />

the instrument panel for information that might confirm or disconfirm<br />

whether it really is the oxygen. It is.<br />

“If we were compelled to make<br />

a choice between these personal<br />

attributes [of a thoughtful reasoner]<br />

and knowledge about the principles<br />

of logical reasoning together with<br />

some degree of technical skill<br />

in manipulating special logical<br />

processes, we should decide for<br />

the former.”<br />

John Dewey, How We Think iii<br />

Think <strong>Critical</strong>ly, by Peter Facione and Carol Ann Gittens. Published by Prentice Hall. Copyright © 2013 by <strong>Pearson</strong> Education, Inc.

• ••<br />

<strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> <strong>Disposition</strong> <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Rating</strong> <strong>Form</strong><br />

Answer yes or no to each. Can I name any<br />

specific instances over the past two days<br />

when I:<br />

1 was courageous enough to<br />

ask tough questions about some of<br />

my longest held and most cherished<br />

beliefs<br />

2 backed away from questions<br />

that might undercut some of my<br />

longest held and most cherished<br />

3 showed tolerance toward the<br />

beliefs, ideas, or opinions of someone<br />

with whom I disagreed<br />

4 tried to find information to<br />

build up my side of an argument but<br />

not the other side<br />

5 tried to think ahead and anticipate<br />

the consequences of various<br />

options<br />

6 laughed at what other people<br />

said and made fun of their beliefs,<br />

values, opinion, or points of views<br />

7 made a serious effort to be<br />

analytical about the foreseeable outcomes<br />

of my decisions<br />

8 manipulated information to<br />

suit my own purposes<br />

9 encouraged peers not to dismiss<br />

out of hand the opinions and<br />

ideas other people offered<br />

10 acted with disregard for the<br />

possible averse consequences of my<br />

choices<br />

11 organized for myself a<br />

thoughtfully systematic approach to<br />

a question or issue<br />

12 jumped in and tried to solve a<br />

problem without first thinking about<br />

how to approach it<br />

13 approached a challenging<br />

problem with confidence that I could<br />

think it through<br />

14 instead of working through a<br />

question for myself, took the easy<br />

way out and asked someone else for<br />

the answer<br />

15 read a report, newspaper, or<br />

book chapter or watched the world<br />

news or a documentary just to learn<br />

something new<br />

16 put zero effort into learning<br />

something new until I saw the immediate<br />

utility in doing so<br />

17 showed how strong I was by<br />

being willing to honestly reconsider<br />

a decision<br />

18 showed how strong I was by<br />

refusing to change my mind<br />

19 attended to variations in circumstances,<br />

contexts, and situations<br />

in coming to a decision<br />

20 refused to reconsider my<br />

position on an issue in light of differences<br />

in context, situations, or<br />

circumstances<br />

If you have described yourself honestly, this<br />

self-rating form can offer a rough estimate<br />

of what you think your overall disposition<br />

toward critical thinking has been in the past<br />

two days.<br />

Give yourself 5 points for every “Yes” on<br />

odd numbered items and for every “No” on<br />

even numbered items. If your total is 70 or<br />

above, you are rating your disposition toward<br />

critical thinking over the past two days<br />

as generally positive. Scores of 50 or lower<br />

indicate a self-rating that is averse or hostile<br />

toward critical thinking over the past two<br />

days. Scores between 50 and 70 show that<br />

you would rate yourself as displaying an ambivalent<br />

or mixed overall disposition toward<br />

critical thinking over the past two days.<br />

Interpret results on this tool cautiously. At<br />

best this tool offers only a rough approximation<br />

with regard to a brief moment in<br />

time. Other tools are more refined, such as<br />

the California <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> <strong>Disposition</strong><br />

Inventory, which gives results for each of<br />

the seven critical thinking habits of mind.<br />

© 2009 Measured Reasons LLC, Hermosa<br />

Beach, CA. Used with permission.<br />

21<br />

Skilled and Eager to Think<br />

Being by habit inclined to anticipate consequences, everyone<br />

silently contemplates the potential tragedy implied by the<br />

loss of oxygen. As truth-seekers, they must accept the finding.<br />

They cannot fathom denying it or hiding from it. Their somber<br />

response comes in the form of Mission Control’s grim but objective<br />

acknowledgment that the spacecraft is venting.<br />

OK, now we have the truth. What are we going to do about<br />

it The characters depicted in this movie are driven by a powerful<br />

orientation toward using critical thinking to resolve whatever<br />

problems they encounter. The room erupts with noise<br />

as each person refocuses on their little piece of the problem.<br />

People are moving quickly, talking fast, pulling headset wires<br />

out of sockets in their haste. The chaos and cacophony in the<br />

room reveal that the group is not yet taking a systematic, organized<br />

approach. Monitoring this, Ed Harris’s character interjects<br />

another self-correction into the group’s critical thinking.<br />

He may not yet know how this problem of the oxygen supply<br />

is going to be solved or even whether this problem can<br />

be solved, but he is going to be sure that the ground crew addresses<br />

it with all the skill and all the mental power it can muster.<br />

He directs everyone to locate whomever they may need to<br />

assist them in their work and to focus themselves and those<br />

others immediately on working the problem.<br />

As depicted in this excerpt, the combined ground crew<br />

and spacecraft crew, as a group, would earn a top score on<br />

“Holistic <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Scoring Rubric.” The emotions and<br />

stresses of the situation are unmistakable. The group’s powerfully<br />

strong critical thinking habits of mind enable the group to<br />

use that energy productively. It gives urgency to the efforts.<br />

Thus, the message about our thinking processes that emerges<br />

is that emotion need not be the antithesis to reason; emotion<br />

can be the impetus to reason.<br />

Chapter 02 22<br />

THE SPIRIT OF A STRONG<br />

CRITICAL THINKER<br />

The scene shown in the video clip was well staged. Skillful<br />

actors displayed the behaviors and responses of strong critical<br />

thinkers engaged in problem solving at a moment of crisis.<br />

Authors of screenplays and novels often endow their protagonists<br />

with strongly positive critical thinking skills and dispositions.<br />

The brilliantly insightful Sherlock Holmes, played in films<br />

by Robert Downey Jr., the analytical and streetwise NYPD<br />

Detective Jane Timoney from the NBC series Prime Suspect,<br />

played by Maria Bello, or the clever and quick witted Patrick<br />

Jane in the CBS series The Mentalist, played by Simon Baker,<br />

come to mind as examples. A key difference, however, is that<br />

fictional detectives always solve the mystery, while, as we all<br />

know, in the real world there is no guarantee. <strong>Critical</strong> thinking<br />

is about how we approach problems, decisions, questions,<br />

and issues even if ultimate success eludes us. Being<br />

disposed to engage our skills as best we can is the “eager”<br />

part of “skilled and eager” to think. First we will examine the<br />

“eager” part, beginning with taking a closer look at the overall<br />

disposition toward critical thinking. Later in this chapter we will<br />

examine the “skilled” part, the core critical thinking skills.<br />

POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE HABITS<br />

OF MIND<br />

A person with a strong disposition toward critical thinking has<br />

the consistent internal motivation to engage problems and<br />

make decisions by using critical thinking. iv Operationally this<br />

means three things: The person consistently values critical<br />

thinking, believes that using critical thinking skills offers the<br />

greatest promise for reaching good judgments, and intends to<br />

approach problems and decisions by applying critical thinking<br />

skills as best as he or she can. This combination of values, beliefs,<br />

and intentions forms the habits of mind that dispose the<br />

person toward critical thinking. v<br />

Someone strongly disposed toward critical thinking would<br />

probably agree with the following statements:<br />

• “I hate talk shows where people shout their opinions but<br />

never give any reasons at all.”<br />

• “Figuring out what people really mean by what they say is<br />

important to me.”<br />

• “I always do better in jobs where I’m expected to think<br />

things out for myself.”<br />

• “I hold off making decisions until I have thought through<br />

my options.”<br />

• “Rather than relying on someone else’s notes, I prefer to<br />

read the material myself.”<br />

• “I try to see the merit in another’s opinion, even if I reject it later.”<br />

• “Even if a problem is tougher than I expected, I will keep<br />

working on it.”<br />

• “Making intelligent decisions is more important than<br />

winning arguments.”<br />

Persons who display a strong positive disposition toward<br />

critical thinking are described in the literature as “having<br />

a critical spirit,” or as people who are “mindful,” “reflective,”<br />

and “meta-cognitive.” These expressions give a person credit<br />

for consistently applying their critical thinking skills to whatever<br />

problem, question, or issue is at hand. People with a<br />

critical spirit tend to ask good questions, probe deeply for<br />

the truth, inquire fully into matters, and strive to anticipate the<br />

consequences of various options. In real life our skills may or<br />

may not be strong enough, our knowledge may or may not be<br />

adequate to the task at hand. The problem may or may not be<br />

too difficult for us. Forces beyond our control might or might<br />

not determine the actual outcome. None of that cancels out<br />

the positive critical thinking habits of mind with which strong<br />

critical thinkers strive to approach the problems life sends<br />

their way.<br />

A person with weak critical thinking dispositions might<br />

disagree with the previous statements and be more likely to<br />

agree with these:<br />

• “I prefer jobs where the supervisor says exactly what to do<br />

and exactly how to do it.”<br />

• “No matter how complex the problem, you can bet there<br />

will be a simple solution.”<br />

• “I don’t waste time looking things up.”<br />

• “I hate when teachers discuss problems instead of just<br />

giving the answers.”<br />

• “If my belief is truly sincere, evidence to the contrary is<br />

irrelevant.”<br />

• “Selling an idea is like selling cars; you say whatever<br />

works.”<br />

• “Why go to the library when you can use made-up quotes<br />

and phony references”<br />

• “I take a lot on faith because questioning the fundamentals<br />

frightens me.”<br />

• “There is no point in trying to understand what terrorists<br />

are thinking.”<br />

When it comes to approaching specific questions, issues,<br />

decisions or problems, people with a weak or negative critical<br />

thinking disposition are apt to be impulsive, reactive, muddleheaded,<br />

disorganized, overly simplistic, spotty about getting<br />

relevant information, likely to apply unreasonable criteria, easily<br />

distracted, ready to give up at the least hint of difficulty, intent<br />

on a solution that is more detailed than is possible, or too<br />

readily satisfied with some uselessly vague response.<br />

PRELIMINARY SELF-ASSESSMENT<br />

It is only natural to wonder about our own disposition. The<br />

“<strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> <strong>Disposition</strong> <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Rating</strong> <strong>Form</strong>,” found on<br />

page 21, offers us a way of reflecting on our own values, beliefs,<br />

and intentions about the use of critical thinking. As noted<br />

on the form itself, “This tool offers only a rough approximation<br />

with regard to a brief moment in time.” We invite you to take<br />

a moment and complete the self-assessment. Keep in mind<br />

as you interpret the results that this measure does not assess<br />

critical thinking skills. Rather, this tool permits one to reflect on<br />

whether, over the past two days, the disposition manifested<br />

in behavior was positive, ambivalent, or averse toward engaging<br />

in thoughtful, reflective, and fair-minded judgments about<br />

what to believe or what to do.<br />

RESEARCH ON CRITICAL THINKING<br />

HABITS OF MIND<br />

The broad understanding of being disposed toward using critical<br />

thinking, or disposed away from using critical thinking, has been<br />

the object of empirical research in the cognitive sciences since<br />

the early 1990s. The purpose of this research has been to give<br />

greater precision to the analysis and measurement of the dispositional<br />

dimension of critical thinking.<br />

Seven Positive <strong>Critical</strong><br />

<strong>Thinking</strong> Habits of Mind<br />

One research approach involved asking thousands of people<br />

to indicate the extent to which they agreed or disagreed<br />

with a long list of statements not unlike those in the two<br />

short lists presented above. Using statistical analysis, these<br />

researchers identified seven measurable aspects within the<br />

overall disposition toward critical thinking. We can think of<br />

these as the seven positive critical thinking habits of mind. vi<br />

Based on this research, we can describe someone who has<br />

all seven positive critical thinking habits of mind as a person<br />

who is:<br />

• Truth-seeking – meaning that the person has intellectual integrity<br />

and a courageous desire to actively strive for the best<br />

possible knowledge in any given situation. A truth-seeker<br />

asks probing questions and follows reasons and evidence<br />

wherever they lead, even if the results go against his or her<br />

cherished beliefs.<br />

• Open-minded – meaning that the person is tolerant of divergent<br />

views and sensitive to the possibility of his or her<br />

own possible biases. An open-minded person respects the<br />

right of others to have different opinions.<br />

• Analytical – meaning that the person is habitually alert<br />

to potential problems and vigilant in anticipating consequences<br />

and trying to foresee short-term and long-term<br />

outcomes of events, decisions, and actions. Another word<br />

to describe this habit of mind might be “Foresightful.”<br />

• Systematic – meaning that the person consistently endeavors<br />

to take an organized and thorough approach to<br />

identifying and resolving problems. The systematic person<br />

is orderly, focused, persistent, and diligent in his or her<br />

approach to problem solving, learning, and inquiry.<br />

• Confident in reasoning – meaning that the person is trustful<br />

of his or her own reasoning skills to yield good judgments. A<br />

person’s or a group’s confidence in their own critical thinking<br />

may or may not be warranted, which is another matter.<br />

• Inquisitive – meaning that the person habitually strives to be<br />

well-informed, wants to know how things work, and seeks<br />

to learn new things about a wide range of topics, even if<br />

the immediate utility of knowing those things is not directly<br />

evident. The inquisitive person has a strong sense of intellectual<br />

curiosity.<br />

• Judicious – meaning that the person approaches problems<br />

with a sense that some are ill-structured and some can have<br />

more than one plausible solution. The judicious person has<br />

the cognitive maturity to realize that many questions and<br />

issues are not black and white and that, at times, judgments<br />

must be made in contexts of uncertainty.<br />

Negative Habits of Mind<br />

After the measurement tools were refined and validated for<br />

use in data gathering, the results of repeated samplings<br />

showed that some people are strongly positive on one or<br />

more of the seven positive dispositional aspects. Some people<br />

are ambivalent or negatively disposed on one or more of<br />

the seven.<br />

There is a name associated with the negative end of the<br />

scale for each of the seven attributes, just as there is a name<br />

associated with the positive end of the scale. The “<strong>Critical</strong><br />

<strong>Thinking</strong> Habits of Mind” chart on page 26 lists the names,<br />

for both positive and negative attributes. A person’s individual<br />

dispositional portrait emerges from the seven, for a person<br />

may be positive, ambivalent, or negative on each.<br />

In the film Philadelphia, Denzel Washington plays a personal<br />

liability litigator who is not above increasing the amount<br />

a client seeks for “pain and suffering” by hinting to the client<br />

that he may have more medical problems than the client<br />

had at first noticed. Watch the scene where a new potential<br />

client, played by Tom Hanks, visits Washington’s office seeking<br />

representation. Access this clip at www.mythinkinglab<br />

.com. Watch the Video on mythinkinglab.com<br />

The clip from Philadelphia starts out with Denzel Washington<br />

talking to a client who wants to sue the city over a<br />

foolish accident that the man brought upon himself. The<br />

scene establishes that Washington is a hungry lawyer who<br />

will take almost any case. Tom Hanks comes into the office<br />

and says that he wants to sue his former employer, believing<br />

that he was wrongly fired from his job because he has<br />

AIDS. You would think that Washington would jump at this<br />

opportunity. There is a lot of money to be made if he can win<br />

the case. Truth-seeking demands that the real reason for the<br />

firing be brought to light. But at this point in the story, Washington<br />

declines to take the case.<br />

Notice what the filmmakers do with the camera angles to<br />

show what Washington is thinking as he considers what to do.<br />

His eyes focus on the picture of his wife and child, on the skin<br />

lesion on Hanks’s head, and on the cigars and other things<br />

Hanks touches. The story takes place during the early years<br />

when the general public did not understand AIDS well at all. It<br />

was a time when prejudices, homophobia, and misinformation<br />

23<br />

Chapter 02 24<br />

nonverbal thinking cues are so well done<br />

The <strong>Disposition</strong> toward <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong><br />

by the filmmakers that we are not surprised<br />

when Washington, having thought<br />

things through, refuses to take the case.<br />

There is no question that critical thinking<br />

is wonderfully powerful. Yet, by itself<br />

it is incomplete. We need knowledge,<br />

values, and sensitivities to guide our<br />

thinking. Washington’s character is<br />

sensitive to what he thinks are the<br />

dangers of the disease and what he<br />

believes (wrongly) about the ways<br />

it might be transmitted. His character<br />

uses his critical thinking skills,<br />

which turn out to be quite formidable<br />

as the film progresses. But his beliefs<br />

about AIDS are simply wrong. He<br />

makes a judgment at the time not to represent<br />

Hanks’s character. It is not the same<br />

judgment he will make later in the film, after<br />

he becomes better informed. Fortunately, he has<br />

the open-mindedness to entertain the possibility of representing<br />

Hanks’s character, that perhaps Hanks’s character<br />

does have a winnable case, and that perhaps the risks associated<br />

with AIDS are not as great as he had at first imagined. He<br />

has the inquisitiveness and the truth-seeking skills to gather<br />

more accurate information. And he has the judiciousness to<br />

reconsider and to change his mind.<br />

surrounded the disease. Washington’s character portrays<br />

the uncertainty and misplaced fears of the U.S. public at that<br />

time. Not understanding AIDS or being misinformed, Washington’s<br />

character is frightened for himself and for his family.<br />

Notice how he stands in the very far corner of his office, as<br />

physically far away from Hanks’s character as possible. He<br />

wipes his hand against his trousers after shaking hands. The<br />

<strong>Thinking</strong> <strong>Critical</strong>ly<br />

1 You can do this exercise by yourself<br />

or with a classmate. This exercise<br />

requires watching TV for two hours.<br />

Begin with a clean piece of paper and<br />

draw a line down the page. Mark one<br />

side + and the other –. With pencil and<br />

paper in hand, watch CBS, NBC, or<br />

ABC or a cable network that shows<br />

commercials along with its regular programming.<br />

Pay close attention to the<br />

commercials, not the regular programming.<br />

Note each of the people who<br />

appear on screen. If you judge that a<br />

person is portrayed as a strong critical<br />

thinker, note it (e.g., Woman in car commercial<br />

+). If you think a person is portrayed<br />

as a weak critical thinker, note<br />

that (e.g., Three guys in beer commercial<br />

2 2 2). If you cannot tell (e.g., in<br />

the car commercial there were two kids<br />

riding in the back seat but they were<br />

not doing or saying anything), do not<br />

make any notation. After watching only<br />

the commercials during one hour of<br />

How Does TV Portray <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong><br />

programming, total up the plusses and<br />

the minuses. Now do the same thing for<br />

another hour, but this time pay attention<br />

only to the regular programming,<br />

not the commercials. Again note every<br />

character who appears and indicate on<br />

the paper if the person is portrayed as<br />

a strong critical thinker (e.g., evil bad<br />

guy +, clever detective +) or a weak<br />

critical thinker (e.g., victim who foolishly<br />

walked into the dark alley alone –). Tally<br />

up the plusses and minuses. Based on<br />

your observations, is there a tendency<br />

or pattern that might be evident regarding<br />

the critical thinking strengths or<br />

weaknesses of children, adolescents,<br />

young adults, middle-aged people, and<br />

senior citizens<br />

2 Group Project – 4-page mini-paper:<br />

Attitudes, while not immutable, are<br />

shaped and formed as we mature. To<br />

the extent that the disposition toward<br />

critical thinking is attitudinal, it can be<br />

affected by our experiences growing<br />

up. Begin by locating research reports<br />

(not just opinion pieces) in which credible<br />

experts report findings based on<br />

solid data about the impact of the<br />

images of ourselves we see on television<br />

and whether those images influence<br />

how we behave. Research on the<br />

power of TV stereotypes, for example,<br />

can be a good place to start. Consider<br />

what you learn through your review of<br />

the materials you found, and drawing<br />

too on your own life experience, formulate<br />

your response to this question:<br />

What is the potential impact that the<br />

characters portrayed on TV have on<br />

the disposition toward critical thinking,<br />

which is developing in adolescents<br />

Explain your opinion on this by providing<br />

reasons, examples, and citations.<br />

In the last part of your short paper,<br />

respond to this question: What kind<br />

of evidence would lead you to revise<br />

or reverse the opinion you have been<br />

presenting and explaining<br />

25<br />

In Philadelphia, the plaintiff, played by Tom Hanks, and his lawyer, played by Denzel Washington, wade through a<br />

crowd of reporters. How does Denzel Washington’s character use critical thinking throughout the course of the film<br />

“The expressions mental disciplines<br />

and mental virtues can be used to<br />

refer to habits of mind as well. The<br />

word disciplines in a military context<br />

and the word virtues in an ethical<br />

context both suggest something<br />

positive. We will use habits of mind<br />

because the word habit is neutral.<br />

Some habits are good, others bad.<br />

As will become evident, the same<br />

can be said with regard to critical<br />

thinking habits of mind. Some, like<br />

truth-seeking, are positive. Others,<br />

like indifference or intellectual<br />

dishonesty, are negative.”<br />

IS A GOOD CRITICAL THINKER<br />

AUTOMATICALLY A GOOD PERSON<br />

<strong>Thinking</strong> about Denzel Washington’s character in Philadelphia<br />

raises the natural question about how critical thinking<br />

might or might not be connected with being an ethical<br />

person. We have been using the expression “strong critical<br />

thinker” instead of “good critical thinker” because of the ambiguity<br />

of the word good. We want to praise the person as a<br />

critical thinker without necessarily making a judgment about<br />

the person’s ethics. For example, a person can be adept at<br />

developing cogent arguments and very adroit at finding the<br />

flaws in other people’s reasoning, but that same person can<br />

use these skills unethically to mislead and exploit a gullible<br />

person, perpetrate a fraud, or deliberately confuse, confound,<br />

and frustrate a project.<br />

A person can be strong at critical thinking, meaning that the<br />

person can have the appropriate dispositions, and be adept<br />

using his or her critical thinking skill, but still not be an ethical<br />

critical thinker. There have been people with superior thinking<br />

skills and strong habits of mind who, unfortunately, have<br />

used their talents for ruthless, horrific, and immoral purposes.<br />

<strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Habits of Mind<br />

BUILDING POSITIVE HABITS<br />

Positive<br />

Truth-seeking<br />

Open-minded<br />

Analytical<br />

Negative<br />

Intellectually Dishonest<br />

Intolerant<br />

Heedless of Consequences<br />

<strong>Critical</strong> thinking skills can be strengthened by exercising<br />

them, which is what the examples and the exercises in this<br />

book are intended to help you do. <strong>Critical</strong> thinking habits of<br />

mind can be nurtured by internalizing the values that they<br />

embody and by reaffirming the intention each day to live by<br />

those values. vii Here are four specific suggestions about how<br />

to go about this.<br />

Chapter 02 26<br />

Systematic<br />

Confident in Reasoning<br />

Inquisitive<br />

Judicious<br />

Disorganized<br />

Hostile toward Reason<br />

Indifferent<br />

Imprudent<br />

To get some sense of the colossal<br />

problems that result from our<br />

collective failures to anticipate<br />

consequences, watch the<br />

documentary film The Unforeseen<br />

(2007, directed by Laura Dunn). It is<br />

the remarkable story of the loss of<br />

quality of life and environmental<br />

degradation associated with real<br />

estate development in Austin,<br />

Texas, over the past 50 years.<br />

It would be great if experience, knowledge, mental horsepower,<br />

and ethical virtue were all one and the same. But they are not.<br />

Consider, for example, the revelations that Victor Crawford, a<br />

tobacco lobbyist, makes in the clip from his 60 Minutes interview<br />

with Leslie Stahl. You can access a transcript of the interview<br />

at www.mythinkinglab.com. He admits that he lied, falsified<br />

information, and manipulated people in order to advance the<br />

interests of the tobacco indu stry. Ms. Stahl calls him out, saying<br />

that he was unethical and despicable to act that way. Crawford<br />

admits as much. He used his critical thinking skills to help<br />

sell people a product that, if used as intended by its manufacturer,<br />

was apt to cause them grave harm. Now, all these years<br />

later, he regrets having done that. The interview is part of his<br />

effort to make amends for his lies and the harm they may have<br />

caused to others. www.mythinkinglab.com and some printed<br />

versions of this book include a chapter exploring the relationship<br />

between critical thinking and ethical decision making more<br />

deeply.<br />

Read the Document on mythinkinglab.com<br />

1 Value <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong>. If we value critical thinking, we<br />

desire to be ever more truth-seeking, open-minded, mindful<br />

of consequences, systematic, inquisitive, confident in<br />

our critical thinking, and mature in our judgment. We will<br />

expect to manifest that desire in what we do and in what<br />

we say. We will seek to improve our critical thinking skills.<br />

2 Take Stock. It is always good to know where we are in<br />

our journey. The “<strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> <strong>Disposition</strong> <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Rating</strong><br />

<strong>Form</strong>,” presented earlier in this chapter (page 21), will<br />

give us a rough idea. If we have general positive critical<br />

thinking habits of mind, that should show up in the score<br />

we give ourselves using this self-rating form.<br />

3 Be Alert for Opportunities. Each day be alert for opportunities<br />

to act on the desire by translating it into words and<br />

actions. Make a conscious effort each day to be as reflective<br />

and thoughtful as possible in addressing at least one<br />

of the many problems or decisions of the day.<br />

4 Forgive and Persist. Forgive yourself if you happen to<br />

backslide. Pick yourself up and get right back on the<br />

path. These are ideals we are striving to achieve. We each<br />

need discipline, determination, and persistence. There<br />

will be missteps along the way, but do not let them deter<br />

you. Working with a friend, mentor, or role model might<br />

make it easier to be successful, but it is really about what<br />

you want for your own thinking process.<br />

The Experts Worried That Schooling Might Be Harmful!<br />

The critical thinking expert panel we talked<br />

about in Chapter 1 was absolutely convinced<br />

that critical thinking is a pervasive and purposeful<br />

human phenomenon. They insisted<br />

that strong critical thinkers should be characterized<br />

not merely by the cognitive skills they<br />

may have, but also by how they approach life<br />

and living in general.<br />

This was a bold claim. At that time schooling<br />

in most of the world was characterized by<br />

the memorization of received truths. In the<br />

USA, the “back to basics” mantra echoed the<br />

pre-1960s Eisenhower era, when so much of<br />

schooling was focused on producing “interchangeable<br />

human parts” for an industrial<br />

manufacturing economy. <strong>Critical</strong> thinking that<br />

frees the mind to ask any question and evaluate<br />

any assumption naturally goes far beyond<br />

what the typical classroom was delivering. In<br />

fact, many of the experts feared that some of<br />

the things people experience in our schools<br />

could actually be harmful to the development<br />

and cultivation of strong critical thinking.<br />

<strong>Critical</strong> thinking came before formal schooling<br />

was invented. It lies at the very roots of<br />

civilization. The experts saw critical thinking<br />

as a driving force in the human journey from<br />

ignorance, superstition, and savagery toward<br />

global understanding. Consider what life would<br />

be like without the things on this list, and you<br />

will appreciate why they had such confidence<br />

in strong critical thinking. The approaches to<br />

life and living which the experts said characterized<br />

the strong critical thinker included:<br />

• inquisitiveness and a desire to remain<br />

well-informed with regard to a wide range<br />

of topics,<br />

• trust in the processes of reasoned inquiry,<br />

• self-confidence in one’s own abilities to<br />

reason,<br />

• open-mindedness regarding divergent<br />

world views,<br />

• flexibility in considering alternatives and<br />

opinions,<br />

• understanding of the opinions of other<br />

people,<br />

• fair-mindedness in appraising reasoning,<br />

• honesty in facing one’s own biases,<br />

prej udices, stereotypes, or egocentric<br />

tendencies,<br />

• prudence in suspending, making, or altering<br />

judgments,<br />

• willingness to reconsider and revise views<br />

where honest reflection suggests that<br />

change is warranted,<br />

• alertness to opportunities to use critical<br />

thinking.<br />

The experts went beyond approaches to<br />

life and living in general to emphasize how<br />

strong critical thinkers approach specific<br />

issues, questions, or problems. The experts<br />

said we would find strong critical thinkers<br />

striving for:<br />

• clarity in stating the question or concern,<br />

• orderliness in working with complexity,<br />

• diligence in seeking relevant information,<br />

• reasonableness in selecting and applying<br />

criteria,<br />

• care in focusing attention on the concern<br />

at hand,<br />

• persistence though difficulties are<br />

encountered,<br />

• precision to the degree permitted by the<br />

subject and the circumstances.<br />

Table 5, page 25. American Philosophical Association.<br />

1990, <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong>: An Expert<br />

Consensus Statement for Purposes of Educational<br />

Assessment and Instruction. ERIC<br />

Doc. ED 315 423.<br />

Putting the Positive <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Habits of Mind into Practice<br />

Here are a few suggestions about ways<br />

to translate each of the seven positive<br />

aspects of the disposition toward critical<br />

thinking into action.<br />

Truth-seeking – Ask courageous and<br />

probing questions. Think deeply about the<br />

reasons and evidence for and against a given<br />

decision you must make. Pick one or two of<br />

your own most cherished beliefs and ask yourself<br />

what reasons and what evidence there are<br />

for those beliefs.<br />

Open-mindedness – Listen patiently to<br />

someone who is offering opinions with which<br />

you do not agree. As you listen, show respect<br />

and tolerance toward the person offering the<br />

ideas. Show that you understand (not the same<br />

as “agree with”) the opinions being presented.<br />

Analyticity – Identify an opportunity to<br />

consciously pause to ask yourself about all<br />

the foreseeable and likely consequences of a<br />

decision you are making. Ask yourself what<br />

that choice, whether it be large or small, will<br />

mean for your future life and behavior.<br />

Systematicity – Focus on getting more<br />

organized. Make lists of your most urgent<br />

work, family and educational responsibilities,<br />

and your assignments. Make lists of the most<br />

important priorities and obligations as well.<br />

Compare the urgent with the important. Budget<br />

time to take a systematic and methodical<br />

approach to fulfilling obligations.<br />

<strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Confidence – Commit<br />

to resolve a challenging problem by reasoning<br />

it through. Embrace a question, problem,<br />

or issue that calls for a reasoned decision<br />

and begin working on it yourself or in collaboration<br />

with others.<br />

Inquisitiveness – Learn something new.<br />

Go out and seek information about any topic<br />

of interest, but not one that you must learn<br />

about for work, and let the world surprise you<br />

with its variety and complexity.<br />

Judiciousness – Revisit a decision you<br />

made recently and consider whether it is still<br />

the right decision. See if any relevant new information<br />

has come to light. Ask if the results<br />

that had been anticipated are being realized.<br />

If warranted, revise the decision to better<br />

suit your new understanding of the state of<br />

affairs.<br />

•••<br />

27<br />

What Is Wrong with These Statements<br />

Each of the following statements contains<br />

a mistake. Identify the mistake and edit the<br />

statement so that it is more accurate.<br />

1 Having a “critical spirit” means one is<br />

a cynic.<br />

2 <strong>Critical</strong> thinking habits of mind are always<br />

positive.<br />

3 If you are open-minded, you must be<br />

a truth-seeker. Hint: open-mindedness<br />

is passive.<br />

4 Calling on people to be systematic means<br />

that everyone must think the same way.<br />

5 If a person is confident in his or her<br />

critical thinking, then he or she must<br />

have strong critical thinking skills.<br />

6 If a person has strong critical thinking<br />

skills, then he or she must be confident<br />

in his or her critical thinking.<br />

7 Truth-seeking is fine up to a point, but<br />

we have to draw the line. Some questions<br />

are too dangerous to be asked.<br />

8 People with strong desire to be<br />

analytical have the skill to foresee<br />

the consequences of options and<br />

events.<br />

9 If a person can see the value of critical<br />

thinking, then the person is disposed<br />

toward critical thinking. That’s all it<br />

takes.<br />

10 People who have not taken a course<br />

in critical thinking cannot have strong<br />

critical thinking skills.<br />

Chapter 02 28<br />

If you feel comfortable with the<br />

idea, you may want to ask a close<br />

friend or two to rate you using the<br />

“<strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> <strong>Disposition</strong> <strong>Self</strong>-<br />

<strong>Rating</strong> <strong>Form</strong>.” To do this your friend<br />

would replace the words I or my with<br />

references to you. This assessment<br />

could provide valuable information<br />

about how your critical thinking<br />

disposition manifests itself to others.<br />

When thinking critically, we apply these six skills to:<br />

• Evidence (facts, experiences, statements)<br />

• Conceptualizations (ideas, theories, ways of seeing the world)<br />

• Methods (strategies, techniques, approaches)<br />

• Criteria<br />

(standards, benchmarks, expectations)<br />

• Context<br />

(situations, conditions, circumstances)<br />

We are expected to ask a lot of tough questions about all<br />

five areas. For example, How good is the evidence Do these<br />

concepts apply Were the methods appropriate Are there<br />

better methods for investigating this question What standard<br />

of proof should we be using How rigorous should we be<br />

What circumstantial factors might lead us to revise our opinions<br />

Good critical thinkers are ever-vigilant, monitoring and<br />

correcting their own thinking.<br />

Core <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Skills<br />

We have talked about the “eager” in the phrase “skilled and<br />

eager” to think critically. Now let’s explore the “skilled” part<br />

by examining those mental skills which are at the core of<br />

purposeful reflective judgment.<br />

INTERPRETING AND ANALYZING<br />

THE CONSENSUS STATEMENT<br />

When thinking about the meaning and importance of the term<br />

“critical thinking” in Chapter 1, we referred to an expert consensus.<br />

That consensus identified certain cognitive skills as<br />

being central to critical thinking. Their research findings are<br />

shown below. viii<br />

Let’s unpack their quote. The experts identify six skills:<br />

• Interpretation<br />

• Analysis<br />

• Inference<br />

• Evaluation<br />

• Explanation<br />

• <strong>Self</strong>-Regulation<br />

THE JURY IS DELIBERATING<br />

In 12 Angry Men by Reginal Rose a jury deliberates the guilt or<br />

innocence of a young man accused of murder. ix The jury room<br />

is hot, the hour is late, and tempers are short. Ten of the twelve<br />

jurors have voted to convict when we join the story. In the classic<br />

American film version of Rose’s play, one of the two jurors<br />

who are still uncertain is Henry Fonda’s character. x That character<br />

first analyzes the testimony of a pair of witnesses, putting<br />

what each said side by side. Using all his critical thinking skills,<br />

he tries to reconcile their conflicting testimony. He asks how the<br />

old man could possibly have heard the accused man threaten<br />

the victim with the El Train roaring by the open window. From<br />

the facts of the situation Fonda’s character has inferred that<br />

the old man could not have been telling the truth. Fonda then<br />

explains that inference to the other jurors with a flawless argument.<br />

But the other jurors still want to know why an old man<br />

with apparently nothing to gain would not tell the truth. One of<br />

the other jurors, an old man himself, interprets that witness’s<br />

behavior for his colleagues. The conversation then turns to the<br />

The jury has the authority to question the quality of the<br />

evidence, to dispute the competing theories of the case that<br />

are presented by the prosecution and the defense, to find fault<br />

with the investigatory methods of the police, to dispute whether<br />

the doubts some members may have meet the criterion of<br />

“reasonable doubt” or not, and to take into consideration all<br />

the contextual and circumstantial elements that may be relevant.<br />

In other words, a good jury is the embodiment of good<br />

critical thinking that a group of people practice. The stronger<br />

their collective skills, the greater the justice that will be done.<br />

Access the El Train video clip from 12 Angry Men at www.<br />

mythinkinglab.com. Watch the Video on mythinkinglab.com<br />

CRITICAL THINKING SKILLS FIRE<br />

IN MANY COMBINATIONS<br />

question of how to interpret the expression “I’m going kill you!”<br />

that the accused is alleged to have shouted. One juror wants<br />

to take it literally as a statement of intent. Another argues that<br />

context matters, that words and phrases cannot always be<br />

taken literally. Someone asks why the defense attorney did not<br />

bring up these same arguments during his cross examination<br />

of the witness. In their evaluation, the jury does seem to agree<br />

on the quality of the defense—namely, that it was poor. One<br />

juror draws the conclusion that this means the lawyer thought<br />

his own client was guilty. But is that so Could there be some<br />

other explanation or interpretation for the half-hearted defense<br />

“We understand critical thinking<br />

to be purposeful, self-regulatory<br />

judgment which results in<br />

interpretation, analysis, evaluation,<br />

and inference, as well as explanation<br />

of the evidential, conceptual,<br />

methodological, criteriological, or<br />

contextual considerations upon<br />

which that judgment is based.”<br />

— The Delphi Report, American<br />

Philosophical Association xi<br />

One way to present critical thinking skills is in the form of a<br />

list. But lists typically suggest that we move from one item to<br />

another in a predetermined step-by-step progression, similar<br />

to pilots methodically working down the mandatory list of preflight<br />

safety checks. <strong>Critical</strong> thinking is not rote or scripted in<br />

the way that a list of skills might suggest.<br />

<strong>Critical</strong> thinking is a form of judgment—namely reflective,<br />

purposeful judgment. The skills are what we use to<br />

make that judgment. Imagine for a moment what it is like<br />

looking for an address while driving on a busy and unfamiliar<br />

street. To do this, we must simultaneously be coordinating<br />

the use of many skills, but fundamentally our focus<br />

is on the driving and not on the individual skills. We are<br />

concentrating on street signs and address numbers while<br />

also interpreting traffic signals such as stoplights, and controlling<br />

the car’s speed, direction, and location relative to<br />

other vehicles. Driving requires coordinating physical skills<br />

such as how hard to press the gas or tap the brakes and<br />

mental skills such as analyzing the movement of our vehicle<br />

relative to those around ours to avoid accidents.<br />

In the end, however, we say that we drove the car to the<br />

destination. We do not list all the skills, and we certainly do<br />

not practice them one by one in a serial order. Rather, we<br />

use them all in concert. <strong>Critical</strong> thinking has certain important<br />

features in common with looking for an address while<br />

driving on a busy and unfamiliar street. The key similarity to<br />

notice here is that critical thinking requires using all the skills<br />

in concert, not one at a time sequentially.<br />

The intricate interaction of critical thinking skills in real-life<br />

problem solving and decision making may begin with an analysis,<br />

an interpretation, an inference, or an evaluation. Then, using<br />

self-regulation, we may go back and check ourselves for accuracy.<br />

On other occasions, we may first draw an inference on the<br />

basis of an interpretation and then evaluate our own inference.<br />

We may be explaining our reasoning to someone and realize,<br />

because we are monitoring our own thinking, that our reasoning<br />

is not adequate. And this may lead us to recheck our analyses or<br />

our inferences to see where we may need to refine our thinking.<br />

29<br />

Chapter 02 30<br />

That was what the jury, considered as a whole, was doing in 12<br />

Angry Men—going back and forth among interpretation, analysis,<br />

inference, and evaluation, with Henry Fonda’s character<br />

as the person who called for more careful self-monitoring and<br />

self-correction. The jury’s deliberation demanded reflection and<br />

an orderly analysis and evaluation of the facts, but deliberation<br />

is not constrained by adherence to a predetermined list or sequencing<br />

of mental events. Nor is critical thinking.<br />

No, it would be an unfortunate and misleading oversimplification<br />

to reduce critical thinking to a list of skills, such as the<br />

recipe on the lid of dehydrated soup: first analyze, then infer,<br />

then explain, then close the lid and wait five minutes. To avoid<br />

the misimpressions that a list might engender, we need some<br />

other way of displaying the names of the skills.<br />

We xii have always found it helpful when talking with college<br />

students and faculty around the world about critical thinking skills<br />

to use the metaphor of a sphere with the names of the skills displayed<br />

randomly over its surface. xiii Why a sphere Three reasons.<br />

• First, organizing the names of the skills on a sphere is truer<br />

to our lived experience of engaging in reflective judgment,<br />

as indicated above. We have all experienced those moments<br />

when, in the mental space of a few seconds, our<br />

minds fly from interpretation to analysis to inference and<br />

evaluation as we try to sort out our thoughts before we<br />

commit ourselves to a particular decision. We may go back<br />

and forth interpreting what we are seeing, analyzing ideas<br />

and drawing tentative inferences, trying to be sure that we<br />

have things right before we make a judgment.<br />

• Second, a sphere does not presume any given order of<br />

events, which, for the present, is truer to the current state<br />

of the science.<br />

• Third, a sphere reminds us about another important characteristic<br />

of critical thinking skills, namely that each can be<br />

applied to the other and to themselves. xiv We can analyze<br />

Core <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong><br />

Skills Interact<br />

Professionals Measure Outcomes<br />

Musicians<br />

Salesperson<br />

Athletes<br />

Nurses<br />

Teachers<br />

Soldiers<br />

our inferences. We can analyze our analyses. We can explain<br />

our interpretations. We can evaluate our explanations.<br />

We can monitor those processes and correct any mistakes<br />

we might see ourselves making. In this way, the core critical<br />

thinking skills can be said to interact.<br />

STRENGTHENING OUR CORE<br />

CRITICAL THINKING SKILLS<br />

Quality of<br />

the concert<br />

Number of<br />

sales<br />

Games won<br />

Health care<br />

outcomes<br />

achieved<br />

<strong>Learning</strong><br />

accomplished<br />

Success of<br />

the mission<br />

Musicians, salespeople, athletes, nurses, teachers, and soldiers<br />

strive to improve their likelihood of success by strengthening<br />

the skills needed in their respective professions. Even<br />

as they train in one skill or another, working people must not<br />

lose sight of how those skills come together in their professional<br />

work. The quality of the concert, the number of sales<br />

made, the games won, the health care outcomes achieved,<br />

the learning accomplished, and the success of the mission—<br />

these are the outcomes that count. The same holds for critical<br />

thinkers. Success consists of making well-reasoned, reflective<br />

judgments to solve problems effectively and to make good<br />

decisions. <strong>Critical</strong> thinking skills are the tools we use to accomplish<br />

those purposes. In the driving example, our attention<br />

was on the challenges associated with reaching the intended<br />

street address. In real-world critical thinking, our attention will<br />

be on the challenges associated with solving the problem or<br />

making the decision at hand.<br />

THE ART OF THE GOOD QUESTION<br />

There are many familiar questions that invite people to use<br />

their critical thinking skills. We can associate certain questions<br />

with certain skills. The table below gives some examples. xv<br />

Often, our best critical thinking comes when we ask the right<br />

questions. xvi<br />

Asking good questions, ones that promote critical thinking,<br />

is a highly effective way to gather important information<br />

about a topic, to probe unspoken assumptions, to clarify issues<br />

and to explore options. Asking good questions promotes<br />

strong problem solving and decision making, particularly<br />

when we encounter unfamiliar issues or significant problems.<br />

Questions to Fire Up Our <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Skills<br />

Interpretation<br />

Analysis<br />

• What does this mean<br />

• What’s happening<br />

• How should we understand that (e.g., what he or she just said)<br />

• What is the best way to characterize/categorize/classify this<br />

• In this context, what was intended by saying/doing that<br />

• How can we make sense out of this (experience, feeling, statement)<br />

• Please tell us again your reasons for making that claim.<br />

• What is your conclusion/What is it that you are claiming<br />

• Why do you think that<br />

• What are the arguments pro and con<br />

• What assumptions must we make to accept that conclusion<br />

• What is your basis for saying that<br />

Inference<br />

Evaluation<br />

• Given what we know so far, what conclusions can we draw<br />

• Given what we know so far, what can we rule out<br />

• What does this evidence imply<br />

• If we abandoned/accepted that assumption, how would things change<br />

• What additional information do we need to resolve this question<br />

• If we believed these things, what would they imply for us going forward<br />

• What are the consequences of doing things that way<br />

• What are some alternatives we haven’t yet explored<br />

• Let’s consider each option and see where it takes us.<br />

• Are there any undesirable consequences that we can and should foresee<br />

• How credible is that claim<br />

• Why do we think we can trust what this person claims<br />

• How strong are those arguments<br />

• Do we have our facts right<br />

• How confident can we be in our conclusion, given what we now know<br />

31<br />

Explanation<br />

• What were the specific findings/results of the investigation<br />

• Please tell us how you conducted that analysis.<br />

• How did you come to that interpretation<br />

• Please take us through your reasoning one more time.<br />

• Why do you think that (was the right answer/was the solution)<br />

• How would you explain why this particular decision was made<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-Regulation<br />

• Our position on this issue is still too vague; can we be more precise<br />

• How good was our methodology, and how well did we follow it<br />

• Is there a way we can reconcile these two apparently conflicting conclusions<br />

• How good is our evidence<br />

• OK, before we commit, what are we missing<br />

• I’m finding some of our definitions a little confusing; can we revisit what we mean by certain things before making<br />

any final decisions<br />

Source: © 2009. Test Manual for the California <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Skills Test, published by Insight Assessment. Used with permission.<br />

Ask Good <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Questions<br />

Chapter 02 32<br />

Using the discussion about shale gas drilling<br />

as an example, practice formulating good<br />

critical thinking questions about the topics<br />

listed below. Look too at the table entitled<br />

“Questions to Fire Up Our <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong><br />

Skills” on page 31 to get ideas about how to<br />

target specific critical thinking skills with your<br />

questions. Write at least four good critical<br />

thinking questions about each of the topics<br />

below. The materials supporting this exerice<br />

are available at www.mythinkinglab.com.<br />

1 Perhaps with more foresight strong<br />

critical thinkers might have anticipated<br />

this problem. But when the onechild<br />

policy was put in place, China<br />

was also beginning to experience a<br />

phenomenon that has continued for<br />

many decades, namely the migration<br />

of young people into urban areas in<br />

search of better jobs and a lifestyle<br />

different from what is available “back<br />

on the farm.” Recently China was considering<br />

a regulation requiring children<br />

to visit their elderly parents who lived<br />

in the countryside. Review the story at<br />

www.mythinkinglab.com and then<br />

formulate good critical thinking questions<br />

about this policy and its the<br />

potential benefits and difficulties.<br />

Consider for a moment the topic of drilling to extract natural<br />

gas from shale. Natural gas as an alternative fuel source<br />

has not enjoyed the attention of the American population<br />

like solar energy or wind energy, mostly because it has been<br />

thought to be in short supply—until recently, that is. It turns<br />

out that natural gas can be harvested from shale rock formations<br />

two miles beneath the Earth’s surface. The technology<br />

behind shale gas drilling involves sideways drilling and a process<br />

called “fracking.” Over 30 states in the U.S. have underground<br />

shale beds and drilling in those regions has made<br />

2 USA Today reported that 40 percent<br />

of all pregnancies across the United<br />

States were “unwanted or mistimed.”<br />

The report was based on a study of<br />

86,000 women who gave birth and<br />

9,000 women who had abortions.<br />

The study itself appeared online in<br />

the journal Perspectives on Sexual<br />

and Reproductive Health. Review the<br />

study itself and the newspaper story<br />

at www.mythinkinglab.com and then<br />

about this phenomenon.<br />

3 The plant called quinoa offers “an exceptional<br />

balance of amino acids; quinoa,<br />

they declared, is virtually unrivaled<br />

in the plant or animal kingdom for its<br />

life-sustaining nutrients,” according<br />

to a New York Times story about the<br />

problems of too much success. As<br />

global demand skyrockets, quinoa producers<br />

and other Bolivians may not be<br />

receiving either the nutritional or the<br />

economic benefits of this crop. Learn<br />

more about quinoa and the problems of<br />

its success by reading the news story<br />

at www.mythinkinglab.com. Then<br />

formulate four good critical thinking<br />

questions about this issue. Let<br />

one of the questions be about the importance<br />

of foresightfulness.<br />

4 The Buddhist nation of Bhutan has a<br />

Commission for Gross National Happiness,<br />

and the head of that commission<br />

has a problem: Domestic violence appears<br />

to be rampant among a population<br />

whose religion abhors any kind<br />

of violence. Review the news story at<br />

from the perspective of the head<br />

of the Commission for Gross National<br />

Happiness.<br />

5 How could a voluntary parent participation<br />

program that resulted in increased<br />

test scores go so wrong That’s what<br />

parents and school administrators are<br />

asking themselves in San Jose, California.<br />

Read the story by Sharon Noguchi, which<br />

appeared in the June 16, 2011 San Jose<br />

Mercury News at www.mythinkinglab.<br />

com, and then formulate good critical<br />

thinking questions to further analyze what<br />

happened and to guide in the formulation<br />

of possible next steps to resolve the<br />

situation.<br />

some homeowners into overnight millionaires. The drilling<br />

has given a boost to the local economies in terms of jobs<br />

and retail sales. But, as reported by Lesley Stahl on 60 Minutes,<br />

accidents and other safety concerns that have people<br />

who live in drilling communities concerned. Those living near<br />

shale gas drill sites are questioning the contamination of<br />

the drinking water by the chemicals involved in the fracking<br />

process. Go to www.mythinkinglab.com to listen to Lesley<br />

Stahl’s full report.<br />

Holistic <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Scoring Rubric<br />

1 Look at the descriptions of each of<br />

the four levels of the “Holistic <strong>Critical</strong><br />

<strong>Thinking</strong> Scoring Rubric” on page 12.<br />

In each, underline the elements that<br />

call out positive or negative critical<br />

thinking habits of mind.<br />

2 Go online and locate two editorials<br />

from this week’s New York Times or<br />

Washington Post. Select any issue or<br />

topic you wish. But find something<br />

that is controversial enough that the<br />

paper published at least one pro and<br />

one con editorial. Approach the two<br />

editorials with an open mind. Resist<br />

forming a judgment about the issue<br />

at least until you have read and considered<br />

both carefully. Evaluate both<br />

using the “Holistic <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong><br />

Scoring Rubric.” Explain in detail the<br />

reasons for the score you assigned.<br />

Before forming an opinion for or against fracking under<br />

residential real estate in your community, as a strong critical<br />

thinker you first would want to know more about this natural<br />

gas extraction method. Let’s try to think of good critical thinking<br />

questions to ask. You might ask “What is known about the<br />

environmental risks of the chemicals involved in the fracking<br />

processes” or “What exactly is involved in establishing a new<br />

drill site in my community” to promote interpretation. You<br />

could also ask “What are the statistics on the frequency and<br />

severity of the accidents and safety violations associated with<br />

shale gas drilling” to promote inference. Perhaps we would<br />

ask “If our community were to permit fracking, what would be<br />

the economic impact of that decision, and for whom, and how<br />

long would we have to wait before seeing those benefits”<br />

to promote evaluation. Or you could ask “Do my past statements<br />

in support of alternative energy policies, or my financial<br />

interests in residential real estate, or my fears for the health<br />

and safety of myself and my family bias my review of the information<br />

about shale gas drilling” to exercise our judiciousness<br />

habit of mind and our self-regulation skills.<br />

SKILLS AND SUBSKILLS DEFINED<br />

The six core critical thinking skills each has related subskills,<br />

as shown in the table below. The descriptions in the table<br />

of each core skill come from the expert consensus research discussed<br />

earlier. xvii The experts provided this more refined level of<br />

SKILL Experts’ Consensus Description Subskill<br />

Interpretation “To comprehend and express the meaning or significance of a wide variety of<br />

experiences, situations, data, events, judgments, conventions, beliefs, rules,<br />

procedures, or criteria”<br />

Categorize<br />

Decode significance<br />

Clarify meaning<br />

“To identify the intended and actual inferential relationships among statements,<br />

questions, concepts, descriptions, or other forms of representation intended to<br />

express belief, judgment, experiences, reasons, information, or opinions”<br />

Examine ideas<br />

Identify arguments<br />

Identify reasons and claims<br />

33<br />

“To identify and secure elements needed to draw reasonable conclusions; to form<br />

conjectures and hypotheses; to consider relevant information and to educe the<br />

consequences flowing from data, statements, principles, evidence, judgments, beliefs,<br />

opinions, concepts, descriptions, questions, or other forms of representation”<br />

“To assess the credibility of statements or other representations that are accounts<br />

or descriptions of a person’s perception, experience, situation, judgment, belief, or<br />

opinion; and to assess the logical strength of the actual or intended inferential relationships<br />

among statements, descriptions, questions, or other forms of representation”<br />

“To state and to justify that reasoning in terms of the evidential, conceptual,<br />

methodological, criteriological, and contextual considerations upon which one’s<br />

results were based; and to present one’s reasoning in the form of cogent arguments”<br />

Query evidence<br />

Conjecture alternatives<br />

Draw conclusions using inductive<br />

or deductive reasoning<br />

Assess credibility of claims<br />

Assess quality of arguments<br />

that were made using inductive<br />

State results<br />

Justify procedures<br />

Present arguments<br />

“<strong>Self</strong>-consciously to monitor one’s cognitive activities, the elements used in those<br />

activities, and the results educed, particularly by applying skills in analysis, and<br />

evaluation to one’s own inferential judgments with a view toward questioning,<br />

confirming, validating, or correcting either one’s reasoning or one’s results”<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-monitor<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-correct<br />

Source: <strong>Critical</strong> thinking skill definitions cited are from APA Report: Expert Consensus Statement on <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong>. (ERIC ED 315 423)<br />

Chapter 02 34<br />

analysis of the concept of “critical thinking skills” to assist students<br />

and teachers in finding examples and exercises that could<br />

help strengthen these skills. But, remember that “critical thinking”<br />

does not refer to a package of skills. Rather, critical thinking<br />

is what we do with the skills—which is making purposeful reflective<br />

judgments about what to believe or what to do. So, make<br />

sure to use this table as an awareness tool, not a map.<br />

A First Look at Inductive and<br />

Deductive Reasoning<br />

The table describing the core critical thinking skills (see page<br />

33) mentions “inductive or deductive reasoning” in the subskill<br />

column of the “inference” row and on the “evaluation” row.<br />

Conceiving of inferential reasoning as being either induction or<br />

deduction has a long and rich history going back to Aristotle<br />

(384–322 BCE). xviii Over the centuries, logicians studying deductive<br />

reasoning developed deductive logic systems that<br />

are useful in fields like mathematics and computer science. xix<br />

Statisticians have developed advanced techniques of statistical<br />

analysis that are useful for the kinds of probabilistic inductive<br />

inferences characteristic of scientific research and<br />

economics. xx But, as you will see from the examples in this<br />

section and later in the book, we all use inductive and deductive<br />

reasoning in making everyday inferences about what to<br />

believe or what to do. Let’s take a quick preliminary look first<br />

at inductive reasoning and then at deductive reasoning. In later<br />

chapters we will return again and again to explore how critical<br />

thinking skills relate to induction and deduction.<br />

NURSES’ HEALTH STUDY—DECADES<br />

OF DATA<br />

One powerful example of research that uses inductive<br />

reasoning is the Nurses’ Health Study. This project is perhaps<br />

the most comprehensive descriptive investigation of<br />

Early Life Factors and Risk of Breast Cancer<br />

“Epidemiologic investigations conducted<br />

by our group and others have suggested<br />

that during childhood and early adult life<br />

breast tissue is particularly sensitive to factors<br />

that influence the likelihood of developing<br />

cancer many years later. For example,<br />

if the breast is exposed to multiple x-rays or<br />

other types of radiation during this early period,<br />

the risk of breast cancer rises steadily<br />

with higher doses, but after age 40 radiation<br />

has little effect. Also, we have seen that<br />

being overweight before age 20 is paradoxically<br />

associated with a reduced risk of breast<br />

cancer for the rest of a woman’s life, although<br />

subsequent weight gain and becoming overweight<br />

after menopause increases risk of<br />

breast cancer in these later years. These findings<br />

led us to develop sets of questions focusing<br />

on diet and physical activity during the high<br />

school years. . . . In addition, to assess the<br />

validity of the recalled dietary data, we invited<br />

a sample of mothers of NHS II participants to<br />

also complete a questionnaire about the high<br />

school diets of their NHS II daughters; strong<br />

correlation between the mother–daughter reports<br />

supported the validity of our dietary data.<br />

health-related behavior ever conducted. Since its inception<br />

in 1976, over 238,000 nurses have provided information. xxi<br />

The details in the box entitled “Early Life Factors and Risk<br />

of Breast Cancer,” report findings based on statistical<br />

analyses of millions of data points. As you can see, some<br />

remarkable, unexpected, and important correlations were<br />

discovered. xxii Measured expressions like “investigations<br />

. . . suggested . . .”, “. . . is associated with reduced risk<br />

. . .”, and “strong correlations . . . support . . .” characterize<br />

the report. The scientists who conducted this research<br />

are presenting probabilistic conclusions. Their conclusions<br />

are warranted because the statistical analyses provide sufficient<br />

confidence to assert that the relationships on which<br />

they report are highly unlikely to have occurred by random<br />

chance.<br />

INDUCTIVE REASONING<br />

Drawing probabilistic inferences regarding what is most likely<br />

to be true or most likely not true, given certain information, is<br />

known as inductive reasoning. Here are some examples.<br />

• When I stop at a traffic light, I hear this funny, rattling sound<br />

coming from under my car. It is sort of in the middle or<br />

maybe toward the back, but definitely not toward the front.<br />

I only hear it when the car is idling, not when I’m driving<br />

along at a reasonable speed. My Dad said once that the<br />

metal baffles inside a muffler can loosen up if the muffler is<br />

old and rusty. He said that a loose baffle makes a rattling<br />

sound when it vibrates, like when the engine is idling or<br />

when the tires are out of alignment. My muffler is at least<br />

nine years old. So, I’m thinking that probably the rattling<br />

sound is coming from the muffler.<br />

• We interviewed three people, and each one was very personable.<br />

I think that the first person had the strongest resume.<br />

But the second person seemed a lot smarter. I liked<br />

the enthusiasm and energy that the third person had, but<br />

We have now begun to examine the relation<br />

of high school diet and activity patterns to<br />

subsequent risk of breast cancer. We have<br />

seen that higher intake of red meat during<br />

high school years is related to a greater<br />

risk of premenopausal breast cancer. Also,<br />

higher levels of physical activity during high<br />

school were associated with lower risk of<br />

breast cancer before menopause. This is<br />

particularly important, as many schools do<br />

not include regular physical activity in the<br />

curriculum, and many girls are now quite inactive<br />

during these years.”<br />

that person never worked for an organization like<br />

ours before. It’s a tough choice. But I’m thinking<br />

that probably the second person would<br />

be the best of the three for us to hire since<br />

innovative ideas are more important to<br />

us than experience or enthusiasm.<br />

• In the past whenever the TV news<br />

programs in Chicago ran headline<br />

stories featuring a sketch artist’s<br />

drawing of a fugitive, the Chicago<br />

Police Department (CPD) hotline<br />

received over 200 phone calls<br />

from people all over the city who<br />

said that they spotted the person.<br />

Tonight the Chicago TV news<br />

programs are going to feature<br />

a sketch artist’s drawing<br />

of a fugitive whom the police<br />

are trying to locate. This will<br />

probably yield hundreds of<br />

calls to the CPD hotline.<br />

• Suppose we imagine electricity<br />

flowing through wires<br />

in the way that water flows<br />

through pipes. With this analogy in mind, it would be reasonable<br />

to infer that wires that are larger in circumference<br />

should be capable of carrying greater electrical loads.<br />

Cosmos series, which you can access at www.<br />

mythinkinglab.com.<br />

Sagan tells us that Aristarchus<br />

“deduced that the Sun had to be<br />

much larger” from “the size of the<br />

Earth’s shadow on the Moon<br />

during a lunar eclipse.” Aristarchus<br />

used deduction to infer<br />

that the Sun was much larger<br />

than the Earth because there<br />

was no other possible explanation<br />

for the size of the shadow<br />

of the Earth on the Moon during<br />

a lunar eclipse than that<br />

the shadow is being made by a<br />

hugely larger source of light shining<br />

toward the Earth and the Moon from a<br />

very great distance away.<br />

DEDUCTIVE<br />

REASONING<br />

Drawing inferences in which it<br />

appears that the conclusion cannot<br />

possibly be false if all of the premises are true is called<br />

deductive reasoning.<br />

Here are some examples:<br />

35<br />

Inductive reasoning is used when we are trying to diagnose<br />

what the problem might be or deciding which of several promising<br />

options would be the most reasonable to select. Scientists<br />

use inductive methods, such as experimentation, and<br />

inductive tools, such as statistics. The Nurses’ Health Study<br />

report was an example of scientific findings derived inductively.<br />

When we base our predictions on our past experiences<br />

about how things will happen in the future, we are using inductive<br />

reasoning. Reasoning by analogy, exemplified in the<br />

example about electricity being like water, is inductive. In<br />

strong, inductive reasoning, the evidence at hand gives us a<br />

reasonable assurance that the conclusion we are drawing is<br />

probably true. As long as there is the possibility that all the<br />

reasons for a claim could be true and yet the claim itself could<br />

turn out to be false, we are in the realm of inductive reasoning.<br />

COSMOS VS. CHAOS<br />

The idea that the earth is a planet revolving around the sun is<br />

often attributed to the fifteenth-century Polish astronomer-priest<br />

named Copernicus. But, in fact, the first scientist known to have<br />

reasoned to that view of the solar system was the Greek astronomer<br />

mathematician Aristarchus, who lived more than two<br />

millennia earlier. Centuries before telescopes were invented,<br />

Aristarchus had only his eyes and, of course, his mind. Carl Sagan<br />

describes the reasoning Aristarchus used in a clip from the<br />

• San Francisco is west of Denver. Denver is west of Detroit<br />

and Newark. Therefore, we can infer with deductive<br />

certitude that San Francisco is west of Newark.<br />

• Every successful president of the United States was both<br />

diplomatic and decisive. General Dwight D. Eisenhower<br />

served in WWII as the Commander of the Allied Armies<br />

in Europe and then went on to become a successful U.S.<br />

president. Therefore, President Eisenhower was decisive<br />

and diplomatic.<br />

• Either we attended the campus Halloween party last year<br />

or we were in Texas visiting your folks that day. We did not<br />

go to Texas at all last year. So, we must have attended the<br />

campus Halloween party last year.<br />

• Assume that ‘a,’ ‘b,’ and ‘c’ are any three numbers. Where<br />

‘w’ and ‘y’ are numbers, assume that ‘f’ is a mathematical<br />

function such that ‘fwy’ yields ‘z’ where ‘z’ is the number<br />

that is the product of ‘w’ multiplied by ‘y.’ It follows<br />

deductively then that ‘(fa(fbc))’ yields to the product of ‘a’<br />

multiplied by the product of ‘b’ multiplied by ‘c.’<br />

• If God intended marriage for the sole purpose of human reproduction,<br />

and if same-sex couples are entirely incapable<br />

of human reproduction, then it follows that God did not<br />

intend marriage for same-sex couples.<br />

• Not every argument is of equal quality. Therefore, at least<br />

one argument is better than at least one other argument.<br />

What Are Your Professors and Textbooks<br />

Asking of You<br />

1 A good education includes learning<br />

content knowledge and learning skills.<br />

Because there is so much to learn, it is<br />

understandable that many instructors<br />

focus a lot of attention on helping students<br />

get the content knowledge right.<br />

These profs often call on students in<br />

class to answer questions that show<br />

that they know the meanings of technical<br />

terms or have learned the material<br />

from a previous lesson. Sprinkled in<br />

among those questions from time to<br />

time are critical thinking skills questions<br />

like those given in the table on page 31.<br />

Here is your challenge: In each of your<br />

classes over the next two class days,<br />

keep a list of the questions that the<br />

instructors ask students. Then, take<br />

the complete list and evaluate each<br />

question to see which were intended to<br />

evoke the use of critical thinking skills.<br />

Which skills were most often evoked<br />

2 Some textbooks include exercises<br />

at the end of each chapter or unit.<br />

Those exercises can address content<br />

knowledge to be sure it is well understood.<br />

They can also invite students<br />

to apply their critical thinking skills<br />

to that knowledge—for example to<br />

interpret some data, to analyze arguments,<br />

to draw out the consequences<br />

of certain principles or facts, or to<br />

explain the right methods to apply.<br />

Take the textbooks for your other subjects<br />

and review the exercises at the<br />

end of the unit or chapter you are on.<br />

Identify those questions, if any, that<br />

are intended to evoke critical thinking<br />

skills. In the case of each textbook,<br />

write five additional “exercise questions”<br />

that evoke critical thinking about<br />

the content of the chapter.<br />

Chapter 02 36<br />

Mathematics, algebra, geometry, and computer programming<br />

rely heavily on deductive reasoning. Activities that require<br />

us to apply strict protocols, rules, or regulations that<br />

leave no room for independent judgment call on our deductive<br />

reasoning skills. People often enjoy games and puzzles<br />

that test their deductive reasoning skills, such as Sudoku.<br />

When we seek to resolve questions and doubts by appeal to<br />

first principles, commandments, or beliefs that are assumed<br />

to be absolute certitudes, we are often using our deductive<br />

powers.<br />

Deductive reasoning is a very important form of inference,<br />

as is inductive reasoning. We require both forms of inference<br />

to be successful in critical thinking. There is a lot more to learn<br />

about inductive and deductive reasoning. We will explore the<br />

uses and evaluation of both kinds of reasoning in more detail<br />

later. Chapter 8 is on deduction and Chapter 9 is on induction.<br />

As a matter of fact, this is a good place to talk about<br />

how this book is organized and how you can use this book<br />

to plan your learning to strengthen your critical thinking skills<br />

and habits of mind.<br />

How to Get the Most Out<br />

of this Book<br />

Our plan from here forward is to use each chapter to emphasize<br />

different critical thinking skills, always remembering to<br />

apply those skills with strong positive critical thinking habits of<br />

mind. To strengthen our critical thinking skills, we will emphasize<br />

first one skill and then another.<br />

But first things first! Chapter 3, on problem solving, applies<br />

critical thinking to something we all care about, namely<br />

your being successful academically. From there we focus on<br />

strengthening specific skills.<br />

We begin with interpretation and analysis in Chapters 4<br />

and 5 when we examine how to clarify the meanings of individual<br />

claims and how to visually display the reasoning we use<br />

to support our claims and conclusions. In Chapters 6 through<br />

9 we will work on evaluation, looking first at how to assess<br />

the credibility of individual claims and then at how to evaluate<br />

the quality of arguments. Strengthening our self-regulation<br />

skill will be our emphasis in Chapters 10 and 11 as we take a<br />

closer look at how to strengthen our real-life decision making.<br />

Snap judgments and reflective decisions are the topics for<br />

those two chapters.<br />

We draw everything we’ve learned about critical thinking<br />

back together in Chapters 12, 13, and 14. In this set of chapters,<br />

which emphasize inference and explanation, as we<br />

focus on how human beings use their powers of reasoning<br />

to acquire new knowledge by means of analogies, inferences<br />

drawn from core ideological beliefs, and inferences drawn<br />

through methodical scientific investigative inquiry. These<br />

chapters explore the benefits, uses, strengths, and weaknesses<br />

of the three most powerful forms of argument making:<br />

comparative (“this is like that”) reasoning, ideological (“top<br />

down”) reasoning, and empirical (“bottom up”) reasoning.<br />

We complete the core set of fifteen chapters with one that<br />

addresses the vital skill of communicating in writing the critical<br />

thinking you have invested in a given question, issue, decision,<br />

or point of view. Effective writing and critical thinking just<br />

might be the two most important things to learn—period. The<br />

two are connected not only in the world of education but in<br />

every professional field and all throughout life.<br />

If your instructor has elected to supplement the basics we<br />

present in these fifteen chapters, she or he may have added<br />

one or more of the four additional optional chapters to your<br />

version of this book. One of those supplemental chapters is<br />

on how social scientists think, one on how natural scientists<br />

think, one on ethical decision making, and one is on the logic<br />

of declarative statements. Building the connections between<br />

critical thinking and each of those different domains was both<br />

enjoyable and interesting for us, as authors. We sincerely hope<br />

that you have the opportunity to enjoy and learn from one or<br />

more of those chapters too. We think each of them, like the 15<br />

we proudly included in the basic version of THINK <strong>Critical</strong>ly,<br />

will further strengthen your critical thinking skills and habits of<br />

mind. And that, after all, is our the whole point. It’s about you,<br />

building up your critical thinking skills and habits of mind, and<br />

you capitalizing on your critical thinking to achieve success in<br />

college and throughout your whole life.<br />

The key to getting the most out of this book is to practice<br />

your critical thinking skills as often as possible. There<br />

are literally hundreds of exercises in this text. Look for them<br />

not only at the ends chapters but in the “<strong>Thinking</strong> <strong>Critical</strong>ly”<br />

boxes within each chapter. There are more exercises for<br />

you at www.mythinkinglab.com. You’ll find that some of<br />

these exercises ask you to think to yourself while others ask<br />

you to think in groups. This mirrors real life in that critical<br />

thinking is both an individual process on some occasions<br />

and a group problem-solving and a group decision-making<br />

process on other occasions. The point of including all of<br />

these exercises is that it is not enough to simply read about<br />

critical thinking as a topic, or passively view others thinking<br />

critically. We humans learn by doing. Building expertise at<br />

reflective judgment—that is, at critical thinking—takes practice,<br />

and lots of it! Explore the Concept on mythinkinglab.com<br />

37<br />

02 REVIEW Study<br />

and Review on mythinkinglab.com<br />

Chapter 02 38<br />

We defined critical thinking and inquired into its value<br />

for ourselves individually and for society in Chapter 1. In<br />

this chapter we analyzed first the “eager” and then the<br />

“skilled” parts of the phrase, “skilled and eager to think<br />

critically.” The willingness comes in the form of the seven<br />

positive habits of mind that dispose us toward engaging<br />

problems and making decisions using our critical thinking.<br />

For each of these seven habits, there is an opposite<br />

habit of mind that disposes us to be averse or hostile toward<br />

using critical thinking as our way of problem solving<br />

and decision making. We can fortify our critical thinking<br />

habits of mind by valuing critical thinking, taking stock of<br />

our current disposition toward critical thinking, looking for<br />

opportunities to translate the habits into practice, and being<br />

persistent in our efforts to engage problems using our<br />

Being skilled at thinking is only part of the story. We<br />

examined the six core critical thinking skills: interpretation,<br />

analysis, inference, evaluation, explanation, and<br />

self-regulation. By asking good questions, we fire up<br />

those skills and their subskills. The skill of inference included<br />

both of the domains that are traditionally known<br />

as “inductive reasoning” and “deductive reasoning.” To<br />

clarify the difference between induction and deduction<br />

we pointed to examples and presented definitions. But<br />

because this was only our first look at induction and deduction,<br />

we did not go as deeply into these topics here<br />

as we will in later chapters.<br />

Knowing the plan for one’s learning provides benchmarks<br />

for progress and helps a person to clarify his or<br />

her expectations. If the goal of this book is to assist you<br />

in building up your critical thinking skills and habits of<br />

mind, then there had better be a plan for achieving that<br />

goal. There is a plan, and we wanted to share it with you<br />

early in the book. We will first look at problem solving,<br />

then delve deeper into the different critical thinking skills,<br />

with the reminder to always apply those skills with strong<br />

positive critical thinking habits of mind.<br />

KEY TERMS<br />

CHAPTER REVIEW<br />

truth-seeking means that a person has intellectual integrity and a<br />

courageous desire to actively strive for the best possible knowledge<br />

in any given situation. A truth-seeker asks probing questions and<br />

follows reasons and evidence wherever they lead, even if the results<br />

go against his or her cherished beliefs. (p. 23)<br />

open-minded means that a person is tolerant of divergent views<br />

and sensitive to the possibility of his or her own possible biases.<br />

An open-minded person respects the right of others to have different<br />

opinions. (p. 23)<br />

analytical means that a person is habitually alert to potential problems<br />

and vigilant in anticipating consequences and trying to foresee<br />

short-term and long-term outcomes of events, decisions, and actions.<br />

“Foresightful” is another word for what “analytical” means here. (p. 23)<br />

systematic means that a person consistently endeavors to take<br />

an organized and thorough approach to identifying and resolving<br />

problems. A systematic person is orderly, focused, persistent, and<br />

diligent in his or her approach to problem solving, learning,<br />

and inquiry. (p. 23)<br />

confident in reasoning means that a person is trustful of his or<br />

her own reasoning skills to yield good judgments. A person’s or a<br />

group’s confidence in their own critical thinking may or may not be<br />

warranted, which is another matter. (p. 23)<br />

inquisitive means that a person habitually strives to be wellinformed,<br />

wants to know how things work, and seeks to learn new<br />

things about a wide range of topics, even if the immediate utility of<br />

knowing those things is not directly evident. An inquisitive person<br />

has a strong sense of intellectual curiosity. (p. 23)<br />

judicious means that a person approaches problems with a sense<br />

that some are ill-structured and some can have more than one<br />

plausible solution. A judicious person has the cognitive maturity<br />

to realize that many questions and issues are not black and white<br />

and that, at times, judgments must be made in contexts of uncertainty.<br />

(p. 23)<br />

interpretation is an expression of the meaning or significance of<br />

a wide variety of experiences, situations, data, events, judgments,<br />

conventions, beliefs, rules, procedures, or criteria. (p. 33)<br />

inference identifies and secures elements needed to draw reasonable<br />

conclusions; it forms conjectures and hypotheses, it considers<br />

relevant information, and it educes or draws out the consequences<br />

flowing from data, statements, principles, evidence, judgments,<br />

eliefs, opinions, concepts, descriptions, questions, or other forms<br />

of representation. (p. 33)<br />

evaluation assesses the credibility of statements or other representations<br />

that are accounts or descriptions of a person’s perception,<br />

experience, situation, judgment, belief, or opinion; also assesses<br />

the logical strength of the actual or intended inferential relationships<br />

among statements, descriptions, questions, or other forms<br />

self-regulation is a process in which one monitors one’s cognitive<br />

activities, the elements used in those activities, and the results<br />

educed, particularly by applying skills in analysis, and evaluation<br />

to one’s own inferential judgments with a view toward questioning,<br />

confirming, validating, or correcting either one’s reasoning or one’s<br />

results. (p. 33)<br />

inductive reasoning is drawing probabilistic inferences regarding<br />

what is most likely to be true or most likely not true, given certain<br />

information. (p. 34)<br />

deductive reasoning is drawing inferences in which it appears<br />

that the conclusion cannot possibly be false if all of the premises<br />

are true. (p. 35)<br />

analysis identifies the intended and actual inferential relationships<br />

among statements, questions, concepts, descriptions, or<br />

other forms of representation intended to express belief, judgment,<br />

experiences, reasons, information, or opinions. (p. 37)<br />

explanation states and justifies reasoning in terms of the evidential,<br />

conceptual, methodological, criteriological, and contextual considerations<br />

upon which one’s results were based; also presents one’s<br />

reasoning in the form of cogent arguments. (p. 37)<br />

FIND IT ON MYTHINKINGLAB.COM<br />

• Dramatic reenactment is not real life, but then neither are socalled<br />

“reality” shows. However, dramatic reenactments can be<br />

very valuable because they often highlight ideas and decision<br />

making in tense situations where uncertainties and risks abound.<br />

The clips from Apollo 13 and Philadelphia certainly do that. The<br />

transcript of Leslie Stahl’s interview with Victor Crawford is<br />

dramatic for what it reveals. Access all three of these clips at<br />

www.mythinkinglab.com.<br />

• When we reference a video clip in the exercises, as with the<br />

“truthiness” clip from Comedy Central’s The Colbert Report, you<br />

can also find it at www.mythinkinglab.com.<br />

• The video clip of the El Train scene from 12 Angry Men is a classic.<br />

Please view that scene and analyze it carefully, noticing how<br />

the argument unfolds and how the<br />

“train of reasoning” is often interrupted<br />

yet always stays on track.<br />

The movie itself is terrific, and we<br />

urge you to watch it all the way<br />

through. Do you think we’ve made<br />

much progress on the issue of racial<br />

profiling in the past 50 years How<br />

about on a citizen’s duty to render<br />

service as a thoughtful and diligent<br />

juror (p. 29)<br />

39 Skilled and Eager to Think<br />

Exercises<br />

SMALL GROUP DISCUSSIONS<br />

1. Consider this claim: “If textbooks used more critical thinking<br />

exercises, students would learn the material better.” What are<br />

the best arguments for and against that claim Do not take a<br />

position on this. Rather, work to develop the strongest arguments<br />

possible for both sides. What additional information<br />

would you need to investigate, in order to ground each side’s<br />

arguments in solid facts What assumptions about learning<br />

and schooling are required to make each side’s arguments as<br />

strong as possible Again, do not evaluate (yet).<br />

2. Consider this claim: “Professors should ask content questions,<br />

not critical thinking questions. It’s the responsibility of<br />

the professor to lay out the content, but it’s the job of the<br />

student to think critically about the subject matter.” What are<br />

the best arguments for and against that claim As with the<br />

question above, do not take a position on this. Rather, work<br />

to develop the strongest arguments possible for both sides.<br />

What additional information would you need to investigate, in<br />

order to ground each side’s arguments in solid facts What<br />

assumptions about learning and schooling are required to<br />

make each side’s arguments as strong as possible Again, do<br />

not evaluate (yet).<br />

Chapter 02 40<br />

REFLECTIVE LOG<br />

1. Mark Twain is reported to have said, “I have never let my<br />

schooling interfere with my education.” xxiii Connect that sentiment<br />

with the information on page 27 in the box, “The Experts<br />

Worried That School Might Be Harmful!”. What is your reasoned<br />

opinion on the matter If you were critical of schooling,<br />

what would you recommend be done to improve it What evidence<br />

do you have that your suggestions would actually work<br />

in the real world Now ask someone who is 10 years younger<br />

than you what Mark Twain meant. Note the response in your<br />

log. Then ask someone who is at least 20 years older than you<br />

what Twain’s saying might mean. Log the response. Compare<br />

the three opinions: yours, the younger person’s, and the older<br />

person’s. End this notation in your log by reflecting on these<br />

final questions: Should K-12 schooling be designed to prevent<br />

students from learning to think critically for themselves Why<br />

or why not<br />

2. You were specifically asked not to “defend,” “evaluate,” or to<br />

“argue for” one side or the other in the previous two exercise<br />

items. Here’s your new challenge: Keeping an open mind and<br />

maybe stirring up a bit of courage, too, interview two professors<br />

and two students not in your critical thinking class. Present<br />

them with the same two claims, but invite them to agree or<br />

disagree with each one and to give their reasons. Note their<br />

reasons respectfully, and ask follow-up questions aimed at<br />

evoking more critical thinking. You should be able to base your<br />

follow-up questions on the group work you did earlier when<br />

you developed the best arguments for and against each claim.<br />

Then, in your reflective log, record the conversations and highlight<br />

some of the places where the people you interviewed did,<br />

in fact, engage in some deeper critical thinking about the topic.<br />

3. Using the “Holistic <strong>Critical</strong> <strong>Thinking</strong> Scoring Rubric” from<br />

Chapter 1, how would you evaluate the critical thinking<br />

displayed by each of the four people you interviewed Quote<br />

some of the things each side said that led you to evaluate them<br />

in the way that you did. [We know you caught it, but just in<br />

case you didn’t, that was another critical thinking skills question.<br />

This one asked you to explain the evidence you used for<br />

your evaluation.]<br />

GROUP EXERCISE: WHAT WOULD IT BE LIKE<br />

Our habitual attitudes affect our behavior and the way that we<br />

interact with one another. People who are habitually intellectually<br />

dishonest, intolerant, or indifferent act differently in household and<br />

workplace settings than those who have opposite, positive habits.<br />

This exercise invites you to draw on your experience to describe<br />

what it would be like to interact regularly with a person with negative<br />

critical thinking habits of mind.<br />

Scenario #1. You have a brother, close to your age, who is<br />

habitually intellectually dishonest, intolerant, and imprudent in<br />

making decisions. He has been like this since junior high school,<br />

and he recently enrolled at your college. Now he wants to share<br />

your apartment, borrow your car, and get you to help him with his<br />

academic assignments. What is it like to have this person as your<br />

family member Given that you have the power to say “no” to his<br />

requests, what are your plans with regard to his requests<br />

Scenario #2. You have a part-time job in a department store as a<br />

clerk. Your old manager used to let your group solve a lot of the store’s<br />

own problems, like who is going to cover a shift if someone can’t work<br />

on a given day. But now you have a new manager. This person makes<br />

scheduling decisions arbitrarily, and is disorganized, and this means<br />

that your group always seems to be rushing to meet deadlines. She<br />

also habitually does not think about the consequences of her actions.<br />

What is it like to work for this supervisor Given that you have the right<br />

to complain to management about your new supervisor, is that an<br />

option you will pursue What other plans might you make to help you<br />

cope with the approach taken by this new supervisor<br />

THE COLBERT REPORT<br />

Watch the Video on mythinkinglab.com Stephen Colbert,<br />

a master of humor and irony, offers “truthiness” as his word of<br />

the day. How does “truthiness,” as Mr. Colbert defines it, relate to<br />

“truth-seeking” Go to www.mythinkinglab.com to view the clip.<br />

APOLLO 13 QUESTION<br />

Group or Individual Exercise: The scene in Apollo 13 when the engineers<br />

are put in a room and given a task is a memorable dramatization<br />

of a critical thinking challenge. Their task is to engineer something<br />

that will reduce the toxicity of the air in the spacecraft (a) as quickly<br />

as possible because time is running out, (b) using only the things the<br />

astronauts have at their disposal, and (c) using methods that the astronauts<br />

can repeat so that they can build the device themselves. Go to<br />

www.mythinkinglab.com, and watch the scene two or three times.<br />

The second or third time through, focus on trying to identify evidence<br />

of the critical thinking skills and habits of mind. Listen to what the characters<br />

say and watch their body language. Discuss the scene in detail,<br />

and then prepare a brief description of the scene, like the description<br />

that begins this chapter. Your description should highlight those critical<br />

thinking skills and habits of mind you noticed the characters displaying<br />

either individually or as a group.<br />

WEB VIDEO PROJECT<br />

Consider this claim: “Effective writing and critical thinking are the<br />

two most important things to learn in college.” Do not take a position<br />

on that claim; instead present the strongest possible arguments<br />

pro and con. One way to gather information about this is to<br />

ask other people their views and the reasons they have for those<br />

views. Ask at least three teachers or professors, ask three successful<br />

people in business and three in other professions, and ask three<br />

people who graduated at least 20 years ago what they think. Get<br />

their reasons, not just their opinions. Then formulate the arguments<br />

pro and con. Make and post a Web video that shows both sides.<br />

41 Skilled and Eager to Think<br />

  • More documents
  • Recommendations

Principles of Service Marketing and Management, Second Edition ...

Think <strong>Critical</strong>ly, by Peter Facione and Carol Ann Gittens. Published by Prentice Hall. Copyright © 2013 by <strong>Pearson</strong> Education, Inc. 000200010271662400

  • Page 2 and 3: Read the Chapter on mythinkinglab.c
  • Page 4 and 5: • •• Critical Thinking Dispos
  • Page 6 and 7: 000200010271662400 a moment and com
  • Page 8 and 9: 25 000200010271662400 In Philadelph
  • Page 10 and 11: 000200010271662400 • •• The E
  • Page 12 and 13: The jury has the authority to quest
  • Page 14 and 15: street address. In real-world criti
  • Page 16 and 17: Before forming an opinion for or ag
  • Page 18 and 19: that person never worked for an org
  • Page 20 and 21: But first things first! Chapter 3,
  • Page 22 and 23: eliefs, opinions, concepts, descrip
  • Page 24: THE COLBERT REPORT Watch the Video

Extended embed settings

Inappropriate

You have already flagged this document. Thank you, for helping us keep this platform clean. The editors will have a look at it as soon as possible.

Mail this publication

Delete template.

Are you sure you want to delete your template?

DOWNLOAD ePAPER

This ePaper is currently not available for download. You can find similar magazines on this topic below under ‘Recommendations’.

Save as template?

logo

  • Help & Support
  • tuxbrain.com
  • ooomacros.org
  • nubuntu.org
  • Terms of service
  • Privacy policy
  • Cookie policy
  • Cookie settings

critical thinking disposition self rating form

Choose your language

Main languages

Further languages

  • Bahasa Indonesia

Performing this action will revert the following features to their default settings:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!

Geography Department Penn State

  • ORIENTATION
  • INSTRUCTOR INFORMATION
  • PROGRAM HOME PAGE
  • LIBRARY RESOURCES
  • WRITING GUIDELINES
  • ACADEMIC INTEGRITY GUIDE
  • GETTING HELP

Lesson 1.5 Critical Thinking Mindset Self-Rating

Print

Measured Reasons LLC is an organization that specializes in “assessing reasoning, thinking strategically, and educating for critical thinking.” The following activity was adapted from their Critical Thinking Mindset Self-Rating Form. Reflect on your thinking over the last two days and answer the following questions. If you have described yourself honestly, this self-rating form can offer a rough estimate of what you think your overall disposition toward critical thinking has been in the past two days. 

Reflection Opportunity 3

Please complete the following self-rating form.

If your total is 14 ("correct" answers) or above, you are rating your disposition toward critical thinking over the past two days as generally positive. Scores of 10 or lower indicate a self-rating that is averse or hostile toward critical thinking over the past two days. Scores between 10 and 14 show that you would rate yourself as displaying an ambivalent or mixed overall disposition toward critical thinking over the past two days.

Interpret results on this tool cautiously. At best this tool offers only a rough approximation with regard to a brief moment in time. Other tools are more refined, such as the California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory, which gives results for each of the seven critical thinking habits of mind.

© 2009 Measured Reasons LLC , Hermosa Beach, CA. Used with permission.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

How Do Critical Thinking Ability and Critical Thinking Disposition Relate to the Mental Health of University Students?

Associated data.

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Theories of psychotherapy suggest that human mental problems associate with deficiencies in critical thinking. However, it currently remains unclear whether both critical thinking skill and critical thinking disposition relate to individual differences in mental health. This study explored whether and how the critical thinking ability and critical thinking disposition of university students associate with individual differences in mental health in considering impulsivity that has been revealed to be closely related to both critical thinking and mental health. Regression and structural equation modeling analyses based on a Chinese university student sample ( N = 314, 198 females, M age = 18.65) revealed that critical thinking skill and disposition explained a unique variance of mental health after controlling for impulsivity. Furthermore, the relationship between critical thinking and mental health was mediated by motor impulsivity (acting on the spur of the moment) and non-planning impulsivity (making decisions without careful forethought). These findings provide a preliminary account of how human critical thinking associate with mental health. Practically, developing mental health promotion programs for university students is suggested to pay special attention to cultivating their critical thinking dispositions and enhancing their control over impulsive behavior.

Introduction

Although there is no consistent definition of critical thinking (CT), it is usually described as “purposeful, self-regulatory judgment that results in interpretation, analysis, evaluation, and inference, as well as explanations of the evidential, conceptual, methodological, criteriological, or contextual considerations that judgment is based upon” (Facione, 1990 , p. 2). This suggests that CT is a combination of skills and dispositions. The skill aspect mainly refers to higher-order cognitive skills such as inference, analysis, and evaluation, while the disposition aspect represents one's consistent motivation and willingness to use CT skills (Dwyer, 2017 ). An increasing number of studies have indicated that CT plays crucial roles in the activities of university students such as their academic performance (e.g., Ghanizadeh, 2017 ; Ren et al., 2020 ), professional work (e.g., Barry et al., 2020 ), and even the ability to cope with life events (e.g., Butler et al., 2017 ). An area that has received less attention is how critical thinking relates to impulsivity and mental health. This study aimed to clarify the relationship between CT (which included both CT skill and CT disposition), impulsivity, and mental health among university students.

Relationship Between Critical Thinking and Mental Health

Associating critical thinking with mental health is not without reason, since theories of psychotherapy have long stressed a linkage between mental problems and dysfunctional thinking (Gilbert, 2003 ; Gambrill, 2005 ; Cuijpers, 2019 ). Proponents of cognitive behavioral therapy suggest that the interpretation by people of a situation affects their emotional, behavioral, and physiological reactions. Those with mental problems are inclined to bias or heuristic thinking and are more likely to misinterpret neutral or even positive situations (Hollon and Beck, 2013 ). Therefore, a main goal of cognitive behavioral therapy is to overcome biased thinking and change maladaptive beliefs via cognitive modification skills such as objective understanding of one's cognitive distortions, analyzing evidence for and against one's automatic thinking, or testing the effect of an alternative way of thinking. Achieving these therapeutic goals requires the involvement of critical thinking, such as the willingness and ability to critically analyze one's thoughts and evaluate evidence and arguments independently of one's prior beliefs. In addition to theoretical underpinnings, characteristics of university students also suggest a relationship between CT and mental health. University students are a risky population in terms of mental health. They face many normative transitions (e.g., social and romantic relationships, important exams, financial pressures), which are stressful (Duffy et al., 2019 ). In particular, the risk increases when students experience academic failure (Lee et al., 2008 ; Mamun et al., 2021 ). Hong et al. ( 2010 ) found that the stress in Chinese college students was primarily related to academic, personal, and negative life events. However, university students are also a population with many resources to work on. Critical thinking can be considered one of the important resources that students are able to use (Stupple et al., 2017 ). Both CT skills and CT disposition are valuable qualities for college students to possess (Facione, 1990 ). There is evidence showing that students with a higher level of CT are more successful in terms of academic performance (Ghanizadeh, 2017 ; Ren et al., 2020 ), and that they are better at coping with stressful events (Butler et al., 2017 ). This suggests that that students with higher CT are less likely to suffer from mental problems.

Empirical research has reported an association between CT and mental health among college students (Suliman and Halabi, 2007 ; Kargar et al., 2013 ; Yoshinori and Marcus, 2013 ; Chen and Hwang, 2020 ; Ugwuozor et al., 2021 ). Most of these studies focused on the relationship between CT disposition and mental health. For example, Suliman and Halabi ( 2007 ) reported that the CT disposition of nursing students was positively correlated with their self-esteem, but was negatively correlated with their state anxiety. There is also a research study demonstrating that CT disposition influenced the intensity of worry in college students either by increasing their responsibility to continue thinking or by enhancing the detached awareness of negative thoughts (Yoshinori and Marcus, 2013 ). Regarding the relationship between CT ability and mental health, although there has been no direct evidence, there were educational programs examining the effect of teaching CT skills on the mental health of adolescents (Kargar et al., 2013 ). The results showed that teaching CT skills decreased somatic symptoms, anxiety, depression, and insomnia in adolescents. Another recent CT skill intervention also found a significant reduction in mental stress among university students, suggesting an association between CT skills and mental health (Ugwuozor et al., 2021 ).

The above research provides preliminary evidence in favor of the relationship between CT and mental health, in line with theories of CT and psychotherapy. However, previous studies have focused solely on the disposition aspect of CT, and its link with mental health. The ability aspect of CT has been largely overlooked in examining its relationship with mental health. Moreover, although the link between CT and mental health has been reported, it remains unknown how CT (including skill and disposition) is associated with mental health.

Impulsivity as a Potential Mediator Between Critical Thinking and Mental Health

One important factor suggested by previous research in accounting for the relationship between CT and mental health is impulsivity. Impulsivity is recognized as a pattern of action without regard to consequences. Patton et al. ( 1995 ) proposed that impulsivity is a multi-faceted construct that consists of three behavioral factors, namely, non-planning impulsiveness, referring to making a decision without careful forethought; motor impulsiveness, referring to acting on the spur of the moment; and attentional impulsiveness, referring to one's inability to focus on the task at hand. Impulsivity is prominent in clinical problems associated with psychiatric disorders (Fortgang et al., 2016 ). A number of mental problems are associated with increased impulsivity that is likely to aggravate clinical illnesses (Leclair et al., 2020 ). Moreover, a lack of CT is correlated with poor impulse control (Franco et al., 2017 ). Applications of CT may reduce impulsive behaviors caused by heuristic and biased thinking when one makes a decision (West et al., 2008 ). For example, Gregory ( 1991 ) suggested that CT skills enhance the ability of children to anticipate the health or safety consequences of a decision. Given this, those with high levels of CT are expected to take a rigorous attitude about the consequences of actions and are less likely to engage in impulsive behaviors, which may place them at a low risk of suffering mental problems. To the knowledge of the authors, no study has empirically tested whether impulsivity accounts for the relationship between CT and mental health.

This study examined whether CT skill and disposition are related to the mental health of university students; and if yes, how the relationship works. First, we examined the simultaneous effects of CT ability and CT disposition on mental health. Second, we further tested whether impulsivity mediated the effects of CT on mental health. To achieve the goals, we collected data on CT ability, CT disposition, mental health, and impulsivity from a sample of university students. The results are expected to shed light on the mechanism of the association between CT and mental health.

Participants and Procedure

A total of 314 university students (116 men) with an average age of 18.65 years ( SD = 0.67) participated in this study. They were recruited by advertisements from a local university in central China and majoring in statistics and mathematical finance. The study protocol was approved by the Human Subjects Review Committee of the Huazhong University of Science and Technology. Each participant signed a written informed consent describing the study purpose, procedure, and right of free. All the measures were administered in a computer room. The participants were tested in groups of 20–30 by two research assistants. The researchers and research assistants had no formal connections with the participants. The testing included two sections with an interval of 10 min, so that the participants had an opportunity to take a break. In the first section, the participants completed the syllogistic reasoning problems with belief bias (SRPBB), the Chinese version of the California Critical Thinking Skills Test (CCSTS-CV), and the Chinese Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory (CCTDI), respectively. In the second session, they completed the Barrett Impulsivity Scale (BIS-11), Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21 (DASS-21), and University Personality Inventory (UPI) in the given order.

Measures of Critical Thinking Ability

The Chinese version of the California Critical Thinking Skills Test was employed to measure CT skills (Lin, 2018 ). The CCTST is currently the most cited tool for measuring CT skills and includes analysis, assessment, deduction, inductive reasoning, and inference reasoning. The Chinese version included 34 multiple choice items. The dependent variable was the number of correctly answered items. The internal consistency (Cronbach's α) of the CCTST is 0.56 (Jacobs, 1995 ). The test–retest reliability of CCTST-CV is 0.63 ( p < 0.01) (Luo and Yang, 2002 ), and correlations between scores of the subscales and the total score are larger than 0.5 (Lin, 2018 ), supporting the construct validity of the scale. In this study among the university students, the internal consistency (Cronbach's α) of the CCTST-CV was 0.5.

The second critical thinking test employed in this study was adapted from the belief bias paradigm (Li et al., 2021 ). This task paradigm measures the ability to evaluate evidence and arguments independently of one's prior beliefs (West et al., 2008 ), which is a strongly emphasized skill in CT literature. The current test included 20 syllogistic reasoning problems in which the logical conclusion was inconsistent with one's prior knowledge (e.g., “Premise 1: All fruits are sweet. Premise 2: Bananas are not sweet. Conclusion: Bananas are not fruits.” valid conclusion). In addition, four non-conflict items were included as the neutral condition in order to avoid a habitual response from the participants. They were instructed to suppose that all the premises are true and to decide whether the conclusion logically follows from the given premises. The measure showed good internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.83) in a Chinese sample (Li et al., 2021 ). In this study, the internal consistency (Cronbach's α) of the SRPBB was 0.94.

Measures of Critical Thinking Disposition

The Chinese Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory was employed to measure CT disposition (Peng et al., 2004 ). This scale has been developed in line with the conceptual framework of the California critical thinking disposition inventory. We measured five CT dispositions: truth-seeking (one's objectivity with findings even if this requires changing one's preconceived opinions, e.g., a person inclined toward being truth-seeking might disagree with “I believe what I want to believe.”), inquisitiveness (one's intellectual curiosity. e.g., “No matter what the topic, I am eager to know more about it”), analyticity (the tendency to use reasoning and evidence to solve problems, e.g., “It bothers me when people rely on weak arguments to defend good ideas”), systematically (the disposition of being organized and orderly in inquiry, e.g., “I always focus on the question before I attempt to answer it”), and CT self-confidence (the trust one places in one's own reasoning processes, e.g., “I appreciate my ability to think precisely”). Each disposition aspect contained 10 items, which the participants rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale. This measure has shown high internal consistency (overall Cronbach's α = 0.9) (Peng et al., 2004 ). In this study, the CCTDI scale was assessed at Cronbach's α = 0.89, indicating good reliability.

Measure of Impulsivity

The well-known Barrett Impulsivity Scale (Patton et al., 1995 ) was employed to assess three facets of impulsivity: non-planning impulsivity (e.g., “I plan tasks carefully”); motor impulsivity (e.g., “I act on the spur of the moment”); attentional impulsivity (e.g., “I concentrate easily”). The scale includes 30 statements, and each statement is rated on a 5-point scale. The subscales of non-planning impulsivity and attentional impulsivity were reversely scored. The BIS-11 has good internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.81, Velotti et al., 2016 ). This study showed that the Cronbach's α of the BIS-11 was 0.83.

Measures of Mental Health

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21 was used to assess mental health problems such as depression (e.g., “I feel that life is meaningless”), anxiety (e.g., “I find myself getting agitated”), and stress (e.g., “I find it difficult to relax”). Each dimension included seven items, which the participants were asked to rate on a 4-point scale. The Chinese version of the DASS-21 has displayed a satisfactory factor structure and internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.92, Wang et al., 2016 ). In this study, the internal consistency (Cronbach's α) of the DASS-21 was 0.94.

The University Personality Inventory that has been commonly used to screen for mental problems of college students (Yoshida et al., 1998 ) was also used for measuring mental health. The 56 symptom-items assessed whether an individual has experienced the described symptom during the past year (e.g., “a lack of interest in anything”). The UPI showed good internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.92) in a Chinese sample (Zhang et al., 2015 ). This study showed that the Cronbach's α of the UPI was 0.85.

Statistical Analyses

We first performed analyses to detect outliers. Any observation exceeding three standard deviations from the means was replaced with a value that was three standard deviations. This procedure affected no more than 5‰ of observations. Hierarchical regression analysis was conducted to determine the extent to which facets of critical thinking were related to mental health. In addition, structural equation modeling with Amos 22.0 was performed to assess the latent relationship between CT, impulsivity, and mental health.

Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations of all the variables. CT disposition such as truth-seeking, systematicity, self-confidence, and inquisitiveness was significantly correlated with DASS-21 and UPI, but neither CCTST-CV nor SRPBB was related to DASS-21 and UPI. Subscales of BIS-11 were positively correlated with DASS-21 and UPI, but were negatively associated with CT dispositions.

Descriptive results and correlations between all measured variables ( N = 314).

Regression Analyses

Hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to examine the effects of CT skill and disposition on mental health. Before conducting the analyses, scores in DASS-21 and UPI were reversed so that high scores reflected high levels of mental health. Table 2 presents the results of hierarchical regression. In model 1, the sum of the Z-score of DASS-21 and UPI served as the dependent variable. Scores in the CT ability tests and scores in the five dimensions of CCTDI served as predictors. CT skill and disposition explained 13% of the variance in mental health. CT skills did not significantly predict mental health. Two dimensions of dispositions (truth seeking and systematicity) exerted significantly positive effects on mental health. Model 2 examined whether CT predicted mental health after controlling for impulsivity. The model containing only impulsivity scores (see model-2 step 1 in Table 2 ) explained 15% of the variance in mental health. Non-planning impulsivity and motor impulsivity showed significantly negative effects on mental health. The CT variables on the second step explained a significantly unique variance (6%) of CT (see model-2 step 2). This suggests that CT skill and disposition together explained the unique variance in mental health after controlling for impulsivity. 1

Hierarchical regression models predicting mental health from critical thinking skills, critical thinking dispositions, and impulsivity ( N = 314).

CCTST-CV, The Chinese version of the California Critical Thinking Skills Test; SRPBB, Syllogistic Reasoning Problems with Belief Bias .

Structural equation modeling was performed to examine whether impulsivity mediated the relationship between CT disposition (CT ability was not included since it did not significantly predict mental health) and mental health. Since the regression results showed that only motor impulsivity and non-planning impulsivity significantly predicted mental health, we examined two mediation models with either motor impulsivity or non-planning impulsivity as the hypothesized mediator. The item scores in the motor impulsivity subscale were randomly divided into two indicators of motor impulsivity, as were the scores in the non-planning subscale. Scores of DASS-21 and UPI served as indicators of mental health and dimensions of CCTDI as indicators of CT disposition. In addition, a bootstrapping procedure with 5,000 resamples was established to test for direct and indirect effects. Amos 22.0 was used for the above analyses.

The mediation model that included motor impulsivity (see Figure 1 ) showed an acceptable fit, χ ( 23 ) 2 = 64.71, RMSEA = 0.076, CFI = 0.96, GFI = 0.96, NNFI = 0.93, SRMR = 0.073. Mediation analyses indicated that the 95% boot confidence intervals of the indirect effect and the direct effect were (0.07, 0.26) and (−0.08, 0.32), respectively. As Hayes ( 2009 ) indicates, an effect is significant if zero is not between the lower and upper bounds in the 95% confidence interval. Accordingly, the indirect effect between CT disposition and mental health was significant, while the direct effect was not significant. Thus, motor impulsivity completely mediated the relationship between CT disposition and mental health.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-704229-g0001.jpg

Illustration of the mediation model: Motor impulsivity as mediator variable between critical thinking dispositions and mental health. CTD-l = Truth seeking; CTD-2 = Analyticity; CTD-3 = Systematically; CTD-4 = Self-confidence; CTD-5 = Inquisitiveness. MI-I and MI-2 were sub-scores of motor impulsivity. Solid line represents significant links and dotted line non-significant links. ** p < 0.01.

The mediation model, which included non-planning impulsivity (see Figure 2 ), also showed an acceptable fit to the data, χ ( 23 ) 2 = 52.75, RMSEA = 0.064, CFI = 0.97, GFI = 0.97, NNFI = 0.95, SRMR = 0.06. The 95% boot confidence intervals of the indirect effect and the direct effect were (0.05, 0.33) and (−0.04, 0.38), respectively, indicating that non-planning impulsivity completely mediated the relationship between CT disposition and mental health.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-704229-g0002.jpg

Illustration of the mediation model: Non-planning impulsivity asmediator variable between critical thinking dispositions and mental health. CTD-l = Truth seeking; CTD-2 = Analyticity; CTD-3 = Systematically; CTD-4 = Self-confidence; CTD-5 = Inquisitiveness. NI-I and NI-2 were sub-scores of Non-planning impulsivity. Solid line represents significant links and dotted line non-significant links. ** p < 0.01.

This study examined how critical thinking skill and disposition are related to mental health. Theories of psychotherapy suggest that human mental problems are in part due to a lack of CT. However, empirical evidence for the hypothesized relationship between CT and mental health is relatively scarce. This study explored whether and how CT ability and disposition are associated with mental health. The results, based on a university student sample, indicated that CT skill and disposition explained a unique variance in mental health. Furthermore, the effect of CT disposition on mental health was mediated by motor impulsivity and non-planning impulsivity. The finding that CT exerted a significant effect on mental health was in accordance with previous studies reporting negative correlations between CT disposition and mental disorders such as anxiety (Suliman and Halabi, 2007 ). One reason lies in the assumption that CT disposition is usually referred to as personality traits or habits of mind that are a remarkable predictor of mental health (e.g., Benzi et al., 2019 ). This study further found that of the five CT dispositions, only truth-seeking and systematicity were associated with individual differences in mental health. This was not surprising, since the truth-seeking items mainly assess one's inclination to crave for the best knowledge in a given context and to reflect more about additional facts, reasons, or opinions, even if this requires changing one's mind about certain issues. The systematicity items target one's disposition to approach problems in an orderly and focused way. Individuals with high levels of truth-seeking and systematicity are more likely to adopt a comprehensive, reflective, and controlled way of thinking, which is what cognitive therapy aims to achieve by shifting from an automatic mode of processing to a more reflective and controlled mode.

Another important finding was that motor impulsivity and non-planning impulsivity mediated the effect of CT disposition on mental health. The reason may be that people lacking CT have less willingness to enter into a systematically analyzing process or deliberative decision-making process, resulting in more frequently rash behaviors or unplanned actions without regard for consequences (Billieux et al., 2010 ; Franco et al., 2017 ). Such responses can potentially have tangible negative consequences (e.g., conflict, aggression, addiction) that may lead to social maladjustment that is regarded as a symptom of mental illness. On the contrary, critical thinkers have a sense of deliberativeness and consider alternate consequences before acting, and this thinking-before-acting mode would logically lead to a decrease in impulsivity, which then decreases the likelihood of problematic behaviors and negative moods.

It should be noted that although the raw correlation between attentional impulsivity and mental health was significant, regression analyses with the three dimensions of impulsivity as predictors showed that attentional impulsivity no longer exerted a significant effect on mental effect after controlling for the other impulsivity dimensions. The insignificance of this effect suggests that the significant raw correlation between attentional impulsivity and mental health was due to the variance it shared with the other impulsivity dimensions (especially with the non-planning dimension, which showed a moderately high correlation with attentional impulsivity, r = 0.67).

Some limitations of this study need to be mentioned. First, the sample involved in this study is considered as a limited sample pool, since all the participants are university students enrolled in statistics and mathematical finance, limiting the generalization of the findings. Future studies are recommended to recruit a more representative sample of university students. A study on generalization to a clinical sample is also recommended. Second, as this study was cross-sectional in nature, caution must be taken in interpreting the findings as causal. Further studies using longitudinal, controlled designs are needed to assess the effectiveness of CT intervention on mental health.

In spite of the limitations mentioned above, the findings of this study have some implications for research and practice intervention. The result that CT contributed to individual differences in mental health provides empirical support for the theory of cognitive behavioral therapy, which focuses on changing irrational thoughts. The mediating role of impulsivity between CT and mental health gives a preliminary account of the mechanism of how CT is associated with mental health. Practically, although there is evidence that CT disposition of students improves because of teaching or training interventions (e.g., Profetto-Mcgrath, 2005 ; Sanja and Krstivoje, 2015 ; Chan, 2019 ), the results showing that two CT disposition dimensions, namely, truth-seeking and systematicity, are related to mental health further suggest that special attention should be paid to cultivating these specific CT dispositions so as to enhance the control of students over impulsive behaviors in their mental health promotions.

Conclusions

This study revealed that two CT dispositions, truth-seeking and systematicity, were associated with individual differences in mental health. Furthermore, the relationship between critical thinking and mental health was mediated by motor impulsivity and non-planning impulsivity. These findings provide a preliminary account of how human critical thinking is associated with mental health. Practically, developing mental health promotion programs for university students is suggested to pay special attention to cultivating their critical thinking dispositions (especially truth-seeking and systematicity) and enhancing the control of individuals over impulsive behaviors.

Data Availability Statement

Ethics statement.

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by HUST Critical Thinking Research Center (Grant No. 2018CT012). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

XR designed the study and revised the manuscript. ZL collected data and wrote the manuscript. SL assisted in analyzing the data. SS assisted in re-drafting and editing the manuscript. All the authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

1 We re-analyzed the data by controlling for age and gender of the participants in the regression analyses. The results were virtually the same as those reported in the study.

Funding. This work was supported by the Social Science Foundation of China (grant number: BBA200034).

  • Barry A., Parvan K., Sarbakhsh P., Safa B., Allahbakhshian A. (2020). Critical thinking in nursing students and its relationship with professional self-concept and relevant factors . Res. Dev. Med. Educ. 9 , 7–7. 10.34172/rdme.2020.007 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Benzi I. M. A., Emanuele P., Rossella D. P., Clarkin J. F., Fabio M. (2019). Maladaptive personality traits and psychological distress in adolescence: the moderating role of personality functioning . Pers. Indiv. Diff. 140 , 33–40. 10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.026 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Billieux J., Gay P., Rochat L., Van der Linden M. (2010). The role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms in problematic behaviours . Behav. Res. Ther. 48 , 1085–1096. 10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.008 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butler H. A., Pentoney C., Bong M. P. (2017). Predicting real-world outcomes: Critical thinking ability is a better predictor of life decisions than intelligence . Think. Skills Creat. 25 , 38–46. 10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chan C. (2019). Using digital storytelling to facilitate critical thinking disposition in youth civic engagement: a randomized control trial . Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 107 :104522. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104522 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen M. R. A., Hwang G. J. (2020). Effects of a concept mapping-based flipped learning approach on EFL students' English speaking performance, critical thinking awareness and speaking anxiety . Br. J. Educ. Technol. 51 , 817–834. 10.1111/bjet.12887 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cuijpers P. (2019). Targets and outcomes of psychotherapies for mental disorders: anoverview . World Psychiatry. 18 , 276–285. 10.1002/wps.20661 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duffy M. E., Twenge J. M., Joiner T. E. (2019). Trends in mood and anxiety symptoms and suicide-related outcomes among u.s. undergraduates, 2007–2018: evidence from two national surveys . J. Adolesc. Health. 65 , 590–598. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.04.033 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dwyer C. P. (2017). Critical Thinking: Conceptual Perspectives and Practical Guidelines . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Facione P. A. (1990). Critical Thinking: Astatement of Expert Consensus for Purposes of Educational Assessment and Instruction . Millibrae, CA: The California Academic Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fortgang R. G., Hultman C. M., van Erp T. G. M., Cannon T. D. (2016). Multidimensional assessment of impulsivity in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder: testing for shared endophenotypes . Psychol. Med. 46 , 1497–1507. 10.1017/S0033291716000131 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Franco A. R., Costa P. S., Butler H. A., Almeida L. S. (2017). Assessment of undergraduates' real-world outcomes of critical thinking in everyday situations . Psychol. Rep. 120 , 707–720. 10.1177/0033294117701906 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gambrill E. (2005). Critical thinking, evidence-based practice, and mental health, in Mental Disorders in the Social Environment: Critical Perspectives , ed Kirk S. A. (New York, NY: Columbia University Press; ), 247–269. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ghanizadeh A. (2017). The interplay between reflective thinking, critical thinking, self-monitoring, and academic achievement in higher education . Higher Educ. 74 . 101–114. 10.1007/s10734-016-0031-y [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gilbert T. (2003). Some reflections on critical thinking and mental health . Teach. Phil. 24 , 333–339. 10.5840/teachphil200326446 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gregory R. (1991). Critical thinking for environmental health risk education . Health Educ. Q. 18 , 273–284. 10.1177/109019819101800302 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hayes A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the newmillennium . Commun. Monogr. 76 , 408–420. 10.1080/03637750903310360 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hollon S. D., Beck A. T. (2013). Cognitive and cognitive-behavioral therapies, in Bergin and Garfield's Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, Vol. 6 . ed Lambert M. J. (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; ), 393–442. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hong L., Lin C. D., Bray M. A., Kehle T. J. (2010). The measurement of stressful events in chinese college students . Psychol. Sch. 42 , 315–323. 10.1002/pits.20082 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jacobs S. S. (1995). Technical characteristics and some correlates of the california critical thinking skills test, forms a and b . Res. Higher Educ. 36 , 89–108. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kargar F. R., Ajilchi B., Goreyshi M. K., Noohi S. (2013). Effect of creative and critical thinking skills teaching on identity styles and general health in adolescents . Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 84 , 464–469. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.06.585 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leclair M. C., Lemieux A. J., Roy L., Martin M. S., Latimer E. A., Crocker A. G. (2020). Pathways to recovery among homeless people with mental illness: Is impulsiveness getting in the way? Can. J. Psychiatry. 65 , 473–483. 10.1177/0706743719885477 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee H. S., Kim S., Choi I., Lee K. U. (2008). Prevalence and risk factors associated with suicide ideation and attempts in korean college students . Psychiatry Investig. 5 , 86–93. 10.4306/pi.2008.5.2.86 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Li S., Ren X., Schweizer K., Brinthaupt T. M., Wang T. (2021). Executive functions as predictors of critical thinking: behavioral and neural evidence . Learn. Instruct. 71 :101376. 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2020.101376 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lin Y. (2018). Developing Critical Thinking in EFL Classes: An Infusion Approach . Singapore: Springer Publications. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luo Q. X., Yang X. H. (2002). Revising on Chinese version of California critical thinkingskillstest . Psychol. Sci. (Chinese). 25 , 740–741. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mamun M. A., Misti J. M., Hosen I., Mamun F. A. (2021). Suicidal behaviors and university entrance test-related factors: a bangladeshi exploratory study . Persp. Psychiatric Care. 4 , 1–10. 10.1111/ppc.12783 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Patton J. H., Stanford M. S., Barratt E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale . J Clin. Psychol. 51 , 768–774. 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)5 1 :63.0.CO;2-1 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peng M. C., Wang G. C., Chen J. L., Chen M. H., Bai H. H., Li S. G., et al.. (2004). Validity and reliability of the Chinese critical thinking disposition inventory . J. Nurs. China (Zhong Hua Hu Li Za Zhi). 39 , 644–647. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Profetto-Mcgrath J. (2005). Critical thinking and evidence-based practice . J. Prof. Nurs. 21 , 364–371. 10.1016/j.profnurs.2005.10.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ren X., Tong Y., Peng P., Wang T. (2020). Critical thinking predicts academic performance beyond general cognitive ability: evidence from adults and children . Intelligence 82 :101487. 10.1016/j.intell.2020.101487 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sanja M., Krstivoje S. (2015). Developing critical thinking in elementary mathematics education through a suitable selection of content and overall student performance . Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 180 , 653–659. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.02.174 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stupple E., Maratos F. A., Elander J., Hunt T. E., Aubeeluck A. V. (2017). Development of the critical thinking toolkit (critt): a measure of student attitudes and beliefs about critical thinking . Think. Skills Creat. 23 , 91–100. 10.1016/j.tsc.2016.11.007 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Suliman W. A., Halabi J. (2007). Critical thinking, self-esteem, and state anxiety of nursing students . Nurse Educ. Today. 27 , 162–168. 10.1016/j.nedt.2006.04.008 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ugwuozor F. O., Otu M. S., Mbaji I. N. (2021). Critical thinking intervention for stress reduction among undergraduates in the Nigerian Universities . Medicine 100 :25030. 10.1097/MD.0000000000025030 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Velotti P., Garofalo C., Petrocchi C., Cavallo F., Popolo R., Dimaggio G. (2016). Alexithymia, emotion dysregulation, impulsivity and aggression: a multiple mediation model . Psychiatry Res. 237 , 296–303. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.025 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang K., Shi H. S., Geng F. L., Zou L. Q., Tan S. P., Wang Y., et al.. (2016). Cross-cultural validation of the depression anxiety stress scale−21 in China . Psychol. Assess. 28 :e88. 10.1037/pas0000207 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • West R. F., Toplak M. E., Stanovich K. E. (2008). Heuristics and biases as measures of critical thinking: associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions . J. Educ. Psychol. 100 , 930–941. 10.1037/a0012842 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yoshida T., Ichikawa T., Ishikawa T., Hori M. (1998). Mental health of visually and hearing impaired students from the viewpoint of the University Personality Inventory . Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 52 , 413–418. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yoshinori S., Marcus G. (2013). The dual effects of critical thinking disposition on worry. PLoS ONE 8 :e79714. 10.1371/journal.pone.007971 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang J., Lanza S., Zhang M., Su B. (2015). Structure of the University personality inventory for chinese college students . Psychol. Rep. 116 , 821–839. 10.2466/08.02.PR0.116k26w3 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Cultivating A Critical Thinking Mindset

Profile image of Peter A Facione

Since the publication of the California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory in 1992, our research team has often been asked what a person might do in order to develop strong critical thinking habits of mind. The answer, as it turns out, is both easier and more difficult than might at first be imagined. This essay offers specific suggestions – practices which can be incorporate into daily living. After a quick summary of how education and socialization are used in the formation of character, the paper provides a summary analysis of the seven positive attributes which comprise the critical thinking mindset, a preliminary self-assessment tool which the ready may elect to use, a discussion distinguishing strong critical thinking from knowledge and then from ethics, and finally specific recommendations for building and sustaining all seven positive attributes of a strong critical thinker. Permission is granted without charge for the non-commercial educational use of this paper with citation as indicated in the first note.

Related Papers

Noreen Facione , Peter A Facione

This invited paper appeared in the Journal INQUIRY which chronicles theory and research in logic and critical thinking. In this , Peter and Noreen Facione describe their passionate involvement with critical thinking, its definition, measurement, training, and practical application to everyday decisions, big and small. In reflecting on their work they say “we have identified three groups of questions: those vexing, recurring questions that motivate us to explore critical thinking, those scholarly questions around which we organized our empirical and conceptual research, and those urgent practical questions which demand the development of applications and assessment solutions. We conclude with two recommendations for the consideration of all those who value fair-minded, well-reasoned, reflective decision making.”

critical thinking disposition self rating form

Peter A Facione

This is the front matter and first chapter of an anthology collection of best practices in the training of clinical reasoning. Invited authors are all published researchers who train clinical reasoning skills in the health sciences. Here they share favorite seminar sessions.

What should I believe and what should I do? Throughout the world, and throughout time, these questions have been applied in the context of specific problems and purposes dominating human thinking. We are who we are, as human beings, because of the answers we have given ourselves. And among these answers is one that says: “We should always go on questioning in the hope of even better answers.” This consistent disposition to question, to ask why, to seek courageously and fair-mindedly the best possible answers to every possible question, lies at the very heart of critical thinking. In this essay we will briefly trace the rise of this consistent disposition toward critical thinking in the Western Philosophical Tradition. From its humble and practical roots in ancient Greece, we will jump forward through the centuries past the emergence of modern science all the way to how we define and measure the disposition toward critical thinking today. Finally, we will complete this story with suggestions about how we might build stronger critical thinking skills and cultivate a more robust disposition toward critical thinking. Because of the broad sweep of our story, the language in this essay will, necessarily, be quite general. But the research and teaching applications are none the less particular and practical.

Spanish translation of the "Case" The X979 Jumpstart" Silicon Valley’s ferociously competitive market for highly skilled technology experts provides the fuel for this business ethics case. The story, which unfolds progressively, tells of the CEO level agreements made and broken, naïve and not so naïve attempts to raid high value technology talent from business competitors, and eventually the vengeful reprisals of the aggrieved rival. This fictional case is intended for use in training and education programs for critical thinking in business and professional ethics.

BRAIN. Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience ISSN 2067-3957

Academia EduSoft , Ali Taghinezhad

This study was intended to investigate the effectiveness of teaching critical thinking on students' writing performance and their critical thinking dispositions. To this end, 140 students were selected. 73 students were assigned to the experimental group and 67 were assigned to the control group. The experimental group received instruction in critical thinking strategies whereas the control group did not. The instruments used in this study were the researcher-developed essay test, the Ennis-Weir critical thinking essay test, and the California Critical Thinking Dispositions Inventory (CCTDI). A 2-group pretest/posttest quasi-experimental design was utilized to determine the outcome measures. Data were analyzed using descriptive statistics and independent-samples t-test. Statistically significant differences were observed in the experimental and the control groups in the total scores of the three instruments. The results indicated an improvement in students' writing performance and their dispositions toward using critical thinking strategies. Nonetheless, some dispositional aspects such as truth-seeking, cognitive maturity, and open-mindedness did not differ significantly after the intervention.

Critical Thinking Across the Disciplines

Jorge Valenzuela

Nursing Outlook

Kevin Crichlow

Principal Investigator and Author - Noreen C. Facione, Ph.D. Collaborating Author - Peter A. Facione, Ph.D. This report summarizes the analysis of an aggregate data set comprising 145 predominantly undergraduate samples contributed by on-site collaborative investigators at 50 programs of nursing education throughout the United States (Total N = 7,926 cases). Collaborators are listed in this report, as are the analyses of each independent sample, for purposes of validation of the reported findings. The data analyzed were collected from 1992 through 1997. Significant relationships are reported between two measures of critical thinking (CT), the CCTST, which is a skills measure, and the CCTDI, which profiles CT dispositions, and a wide variety of academic achievement indicators (e.g. grade point average, standardized test scores), student descriptors (e.g. age, sex, RN-status, NCLEX passage), and program descriptors (e.g. student to faculty ratio, location of program, faculty focus on CT in planning and curriculum development). Modest cross sectional increases and longitudinal gains are demonstrated in CT skills and habits of mind. Several of these relationships are also explored in the limited graduate level sample available. Initial percentile norms for the CCTST are calculated for nursing students by undergraduate class level. Percentile distributions by class for the seven scales of the CCTDI are also reported. A relative strength in students CT skills and dispositions scores were observed in samples collected in nursing programs where faculty reported being engaged in discussions about CT and curriculum reform to optimize teaching for CT. Evidence was observed for a comparable strength in CT skills in both generic and RN completion students on both entry and exit. Disposition scores were higher for RN to BSN students than for same class level generic nursing student on entry. Scores in CT disposition for exiting RN to BSN students raise concern for whether these students’ CT disposition is being nurtured by current nursing curricula. A similar concern is raised in relation to female students versus male students as a result of analyses of observed CCTDI scores by sex at exit. Using this dataset, the largest aggregation of CT skills and dispositions test data known to date, the theoretical relationships between the traditional and the Delphi constructs, in terms of division of CT skills, are also explored empirically, as was the relationship between CT skill and the disposition toward CT.

RELATED PAPERS

Ronaldo Marco Deligdisch

Acta Médica Colombiana

César Augusto Restrepo Valencia

isti mukhayanah

Herman Depypere

Economics Letters

Kajal Lahiri

Carolina Zamorano

Laura Shelton

Revista do Instituto Brasileiro de Segurança Pública (RIBSP)

Luís Henrique Costa Ferreira

Jorge Gustavo Diaz Rojas

Zbigniew Kadłubek

david ramirez

Physics of Particles and Nuclei Letters

Gadir Ahmadov

Dwory i życie dworskie w nowożytnej Europie (XVI-XVIII w.), red. P. Kuc, Kraków 2020

Kocańda Paweł

FSM İlmî Araştırmalar İnsan ve Toplum Bilimleri Dergisi

Mustafa Efe Kahya

Proceedings of the second international joint conference on Autonomous agents and multiagent systems - AAMAS '03

Martin Purvis

Indonesian Journal of Pharmacy

Tutuk Budiati

Applied Microbiology and Biotechnology

Angeles Suarez

New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development

Dinesh Sharma

Revista Brasileira de Medicina do Trabalho

Antonio Sousa-Uva

RIMHE : Revue Interdisciplinaire Management, Homme(s) & Entreprise

Dominique Bencherqui

Journal of Economic Entomology

Stuart Reitz

Automated software engineering

Premkumar Devanbu

American journal of human genetics

Anders Fasth

Sylvain Perret

See More Documents Like This

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Michael Hogan Ph.D.

Intelligence

What are the key dispositions of good critical thinkers, a collective intelligence analysis of critical thinking dispositions..

Posted January 18, 2016

As human beings, we make thousands of decisions every day. Every time we go to the shop to buy bread we must decide whether we would prefer white bread, brown bread, small or large loaf, barrel or sliced, etc. The same goes for the ‘simple’ task of ordering a coffee. For example, if you enter Starbucks, it’s been estimated that there are over 19,000 possible beverages available to you (Vohs et al., 2014). Indeed, we are immersed in increasingly complex and differentiated environments -- decisions are ever-present and surround us as we make our way in the world.

Of course, some decisions are more important than others. Perhaps ordering coffee at Starbucks and selecting bread at the shop are not seen as incredibly important by many people and, naturally, we often depend on prior choices or habitual responses to guide our choices in these situations. But what about decisions we care about, decisions that we recognise as important and that require some deliberation? Repeating habitual responses or ‘going with our gut’ in those situations will not suffice – we must pause for reflection and think critically – we must think about our decisions in these situations. Although it is not always clear what is needed to support good critical thinking, scholars have suggested that there may be a number of core personal dispositions that support good critical thinking. But what are these critical thinking dispositions?

Critical thinking (CT) is commonly defined as a metacognitive process, consisting of a number of cognitive skills (e.g. analysis, evaluation and inference) and a variety of personal dispositions (e.g. open-mindedness, inquisitiveness and scepticism), that, when used appropriately, increases the chances of producing a logical solution to a problem or a valid conclusion to an argument (Dwyer, 2011; Dwyer, Hogan & Stewart, 2012; 2014). However, most definitions of critical thinking (CT), and most interventions designed to increase CT, are grounded in academic or expert definitions of CT skills; and there has been very little emphasis on CT dispositions in the research conducted to date. Furthermore, students and educators are rarely, if ever, asked to describe their perspectives on what constitutes CT or key CT dispositions. As such, critical thinking dispositions have not yet been clearly conceptualised in research literature; and what little is agreed upon when it comes to such disposition is generally derived from the opinions of academics.

So what do students and educators describe as the key dispositions of good critical thinkers? We recently conducted research to address this question, as part of our ongoing efforts to develop and validate a new Critical Thinking Dispositions Scale . Using a collective intelligence methodology, Interactive Management , we examined the way students and educators conceptualise CT dispositions.

Interactive Management (IM) is a computer-assisted process that allows a group to build a structural model describing relations among elements in a system. In the current study, these elements were CT dispositions, and the relations of interest were the ways in which these dispositions support one another in a positive system of influence. Interactive Management is a five-step systems thinking methodology that is used to aid groups in developing outcomes that integrate contributions from individuals with diverse views, backgrounds and perspectives, via: (1) generating and clarifying ideas; (2) voting, ranking and selecting elements for structuring; (3) structuring elements by answering a series of questions in the form: “Does CT disposition A facilitate CT disposition B?’’; (4) evaluating a graphical representation of group logic; and (5) interpreting and evaluating discourse and reasoning to further understand the nature of the collective intelligence. In our study on critical thinking dispositions, three groups comprising a total of thirty-one students and ten educators took part in the collective intelligence workshops. Across these three workshops, a total of 32 CT dispositions were identified and organised into 13 categories (see figure 1).

Michael Hogan, Chris Dwyer, Sarah Quinn

Figure 1. Key Dispositions of Good Critical Thinkers

An analysis of the systems thinking of participants revealed that key CT dispositions including inquisitiveness , open-mindedness and self-efficacy had the strongest positive, supporting influence on other CT dispositions in the set; whereas, reflection and resourcefulness were seen as highly dependent on other dispositions (i.e., supported positively by other dispositions in the set). Drawing upon the logic of these systems structures, and ordering the full set of critical thinking dispositions by reference to their relative influence in the system, revealed the following ordered set:

  • Inquisitiveness : An inclination to be curious; desire to fully understand something, discover the answer to a problem and accept that the full answer may not yet be known; a desire to understand a task and its associated requirements, available options and limits.
  • Open-Mindedness : An inclination to be cognitively flexible, open to divergent or conflicting views; detaching from one’s own beliefs to consider diverse points of view without bias or self-interest; to be open to feedback, accepting positive feedback, criticism or constructive feedback with thoughtful consideration; an openness to amending existing knowledge in light of new ideas and experiences; and a willingness to explore new, alternative or ‘unusual’ ideas.
  • Self-efficacy : The tendency to be confident and trust in one’s own reasoned judgments; acknowledging one’s sense of self while considering problems and arguments (i.e. situating reasoned judgements within one’s own life experiences, knowledge, biases, culture and environment); to believe in one’s ability to receive and internalize feedback positively and constructively; to be self-efficacious in leading others in the rational resolution of problems; and belief that good reasoning is the key to living a rational life and to a creating a more just world.
  • Attentiveness : Willingness to focus and concentrate; to be aware of surroundings, context, consequences and potential obstacles; to have the ‘full picture’.
  • Intrinsic Goal Orientation : Inclined to be enthusiastic towards a goal, task, topic of focus and, if not the topic itself, enthusiasm for the process of learning new things; to search for answers as a result of internal motivation , rather than an external, extrinsic reward system.
  • Perseverance : To be resilient and to be motivated to persist at working through complex tasks and the associated frustration and difficulty inherent in such tasks, without giving up; motivation to get the job done correctly; a desire to progress.
  • Organization : An inclination to be orderly, systematic and diligent with information, resources and time when determining and maintaining focus on a task, problem or question, whilst simultaneously considering the total situation and being able to present the resulting information in a holistic, organized fashion, for purposes of achieving specified goals.
  • Truth-Seeking : A desire for knowledge and truth; to seek and offer both reasons and objections in an effort to inform and to be well-informed; a willingness to challenge popular beliefs and social norms by asking questions (of oneself and others); to be honest and objective about pursuing the truth even if the findings do not support one’s self-interest or pre-conceived beliefs or opinions; and willingness to change one’s mind about an idea as a result of the desire for truth.
  • Creativity : A tendency to visualize and generate ideas; and to ‘think outside the box’ (i.e. think differently than usual)
  • Skepticism: An inclination to challenge ideas and question conclusions in light of the evidence presented; to withhold judgment prior to engaging with the evidence; to take a position and be able to change position when the evidence and reasons are sufficient; and to look at findings from various perspectives.
  • Reflection : An inclination to reflect on one’s behavior, attitudes, opinions, and motivations; to distinguish what is known and what is not; to approach decision-making with an awareness of limited knowledge or uncertainty, recognition that some problems are ill-structured, some situations permit more than one plausible conclusion or solution, and judgments must often be made based on analysis, evaluation, standards, contexts and evidence that preclude certainty.
  • Resourcefulness : The willingness to utilize existing resources to resolve problems; search for additional resources in order to resolve problems; to switch between solution processes and knowledge to seek new ways/information to solve a problem; to make the best of the resources available; to adapt and/or improve if something goes wrong; and to think about how and why something went wrong.

Another way to understand this logic of supporting influence, or the relation between critical thinking dispositions, is by reference to the total influence map in figure 2, where dispositions to the left are collectively seen to influence disposition to the right. This figure represents a meta-analysis of the systems thinking across the three workshops, and highlights dispositions that influence or support other dispositions in the system.

Michael Hogan, Chris Dwyer, Sarah Quinn

Figure 2. Influence Map Describing Supporting Relations between CT Dispositions

Interestingly, one CT disposition, clarity , although highlighted as important during the idea generation stage (i.e., Step 1 above), did not receive a sufficient number of votes when it came to selecting ideas for structuring (i.e., Step 3 above). However, we believe it is important to highlight the significance of clarity , which was defined by our participants as the inclination to seek intelligibility, transparency, lucidity and precision, from oneself and others, and to be clear with respect to the intended meaning of what is communicated .

Octopus hiding behind a sea shell,

We appreciate that more work needs to be done to further understand the dynamics of critical thinking dispositions in action. We are currently working to develop critical thinking educational programmes that support the cultivation of key CT skills and dispositions. The understanding obtained through our collective intelligence research has also informed the development or a new Critical Thinking Dispositions Scale , which we are currently working to validate. We believe a new Critical Thinking Dispositions Scale is needed to empower students and educators who wish to reflect upon their critical thinking dispositions and negotiate new learning experiences that promote the development of CT skill and dispositions in academic settings. If you would like to contribute to our scale development work, we would be delighted if you could take our survey.

Link to Sona version of survey for NUI, Galway Students :

A direct link to survey is available upon request to [email protected]

Chris Dwyer ( LinkedIn , ResearchGate ), Michael Hogan ( Web ), Owen Harney ( ResearchGate ), Caroline Kavanagh, Sarah Quinn.

Dwyer, C.P., Hogan, M.J., Harney, O. & Kavanagh, C. (2016). Facilitating a student-educator conceptual model of dispositions towards critical thinking through interactive management. Educational Technology & Research (Submitted).

Dwyer, C.P. & Hogan, M.J. (2014). An integrated critical thinking framework for the 21st century. Thinking Skills & Creativity, 12, 43-52.

Dwyer, C.P., Hogan, M.J., & Stewart, I. (2012). An evaluation of argument mapping as a method of enhancing critical thinking performance in e-learning environments. Metacognition and Learning, 7, 219-244.

Dwyer, C.P., Hogan, M.J., & Stewart, I. (2011). The promotion of critical thinking skills through argument mapping. In C.P. Horvart & J.M. Forte (Eds.), Critical Thinking, 97-122. Nova Science Publishers, New York.

Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. M. (2014). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation , and active initiative.

Michael Hogan Ph.D.

Michael Hogan, Ph.D. , is a lecturer in psychology at the National University of Ireland, Galway.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Teletherapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Therapy Center NEW
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

March 2024 magazine cover

Understanding what emotional intelligence looks like and the steps needed to improve it could light a path to a more emotionally adept world.

  • Coronavirus Disease 2019
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published: 03 January 2019

Influence of critical thinking disposition on the learning efficiency of problem-based learning in undergraduate medical students

  • Juhua Ni 2 ,
  • Demao Song 3 ,
  • Weiguang Zhang 4 ,
  • Yuedan Wang 5 ,
  • Liling Wu 3 ,
  • Xian Wang 3 &
  • Yun Wang 6  

BMC Medical Education volume  19 , Article number:  1 ( 2019 ) Cite this article

7916 Accesses

59 Citations

Metrics details

Problem-based learning (PBL), a pedagogical approach, is widely accepted in medical education. Manipulated by many factors, the internal motivation of learner is the most crucial determinant that affects the nature of the outcome, in which the influences of critical thinking (CT) remained elusive.

One hundred two third-year undergraduate medical students at Peking University were involved in this study. A Chinese version of the Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory (CTDI-CV) was used to assess the CT disposition, and the performance scores of students in PBL tutorials were compiled. A parametric bivariate correlation analysis was performed between the students’ CT scores and their PBL average scores. The PBL scores were compared between the strong and weak CT disposition groups using independent t-test. The analysis of numerical data was conducted using SPSS 16.0.

CT disposition of third-year undergraduate medical students at Peking University was at a positive level, with an average score of 297.72. The total CT scores had a positive correlation with the scores of the PBL performance and its five dimensions significantly. In the majority, students with Strong-CT disposition obtained higher scores in PBL tutorials compared with students with Weak-CT disposition. The performance of these two groups was significantly different in the Late-Half but not in the Early-Half PBL tutorials. Furthermore, a significant improvement was observed in the students with strong CT but not weak CT dispositions.

CT disposition positively correlates to a students’ PBL performance. Students with stronger CT dispositions perform better in the PBL process and obtain higher scores. Our work suggested that the open-mindedness of the CT disposition is the primary factor that determines the improvement of the preparation dimensions in the PBL process.

Peer Review reports

Following the constructivist theory, the ideal learning process suggested that learner should acquire knowledge and skills actively, and the objective of education is to help students to learn how to learn. Recently, there is a broad consensus that problem-based learning (PBL), a pedagogical approach widely accepted in medical education, has a positive effect on active learning and learning outcomes [ 1 , 2 , 3 ]. PBL is characterized as problem-triggered, student-centered, and tutor-facilitated to achieve active lifelong learning [ 1 , 4 , 5 , 6 ]. Comparing to graduates from traditional curriculum schools, graduates from active learning curriculum schools rated themselves higher not only on the typical PBL-related competencies, for example, the abilities of interpersonal communication, problem-solving, self-directed and lifelong learning, as well as information gathering, but also on the more general work-related skills which are deemed as the crucial aspect for success in professional practice, such as writing reports, presenting papers, doing research, producing new ideas, being able to work independently, being efficient and being able to work under pressure [ 7 ]. Schools adopting active-learning curricula had higher graduation rate than schools with conventional curricula, students graduated earlier than their counterparts from conventional schools [ 8 ]. Overall, PBL aims to foster the ability of interdisciplinary knowledge application, self-directed learning, critical thinking, communication and collaborative skills, teamwork, information searching and management, and professional values. Thus, PBL has become a modern-day educational strategy, particularly in medical education [ 6 , 9 , 10 ].

Many factors influence the outcomes of PBL, including the quality of the scenario [ 11 , 12 , 13 ], the type of learning materials [ 14 , 15 ], the behaviors of the tutors or facilitators [ 16 , 17 ], and the participation of the group members [ 18 , 19 ]. In the point of fact, the most crucial factor of PBL is the internal motivation of the learner. It is generally accepted by the educators that PBL is an active and immersive process in which the students must take significant responsibility for their learning. Students who understand and agree with the PBL concept, who are well aware of and implement the group dynamics, who collaborate closely with team members and who are good at transforming into self-directed learners will garner increased benefits from PBL [ 7 , 9 , 11 , 18 ].

Critical thinking (CT) is usually viewed as a philosophical concept, referred to characteristics of the individual, personality traits or habits of mind [ 20 ]. It is a pervasive and self-rectifying human phenomenon, referring to the dispositions and skills revealing what is authentic, what to believe, why it is and how it happens. A statement presented by Scriven and Paul in 1987 said, “Critical thinking is the ability to apply higher-order cognitive skills (conceptualization, analysis, evaluation) and the disposition to be deliberate about thinking (being open-minded or intellectually honest) that lead to action that is logical and appropriate” [ 21 ]. The consensus regarding critical thinking characterizes it as a self-adjusting process of judging what to believe and what to do in a given context [ 22 ]. The disposition toward CT was the consistent internal motivation to engage in problem solving and decision making.

The consensus was reached that skills and dispositions of critical thinking are positively correlated with the internal motivation to think. CT plays a central role in one’s learning and working, particularly in addition to medical education that transitions from knowledge-based curricula to competency-based curricula. It is crucial for students to learn and work further critically to evaluate existing knowledge and information. World Federation for Medical Education had considered critical thinking as one of the medical training standards. Critical thinking is vital to a health professional’s competence to assess, diagnose and care for patients correctly and effectively. Moreover, the essential requirements issued by the Institute for International Medical Education addressed personal judgments based on the ability to critically collect, analyze, evaluate and apply all sources of information and thus remain competitive in the challenging healthcare environment [ 23 ].

CT is being recognized as a liberating force in education, and a powerful resource in one’s life, increasingly exciting findings were rising in measuring and developing the students’ CT. Numerous studies suggested that CT has a definite relationship with the acquirement of knowledge and the development of professional values and clinical judgments [ 24 , 25 , 26 ]. Studies by Chang et al. indicated that the ability of critical thinking has a positive correlation with nursing competence significantly [ 24 ]. Ghazivakili et al. found that critical thinking correlated with academic performance substantially [ 25 ]. Several other studies also revealed the relationship between critical thinking and academic success of medical professionals, such as GPA, course grade and certification exam [ 26 , 27 , 28 ].

While previous studies most concentrated on following aspects: 1) how the CT disposition was in students, residents or working staffs; 2) the relationship between the CT dispositions and psychological characteristic, such as personalities, emotional intelligence, learning models, or ego resiliency; 3) whether the CT dispositions improved or not after some kinds of curriculum or education reform; 4) the relationship of CT disposition with the academic or career success. Even among the researches in respecting to the relationship of CT and PBL, most studies were concentrated on the variation of students’ CT dispositions or skills before and after PBL, or the different models and elements of PBL [ 26 , 27 , 29 ]. Similarly, the conclusion was reached that PBL could develop the critical thinking ability of students. However, no studies have investigated how CT and its subscales influence students’ performance in the PBL process, namely, all behaviors in PBL process, included the preparations and input, the knowledge, the skills and the attitudes.

The objective of this study is to explore the relationship of CT disposition and students’ performance in PBL and to address the following issues: 1. Does CT disposition correlate to students’ performance in PBL? 2. Is CT disposition crucial to PBL performance? 3. How does CT disposition impact the students’ performance in PBL tutorials?

Participants

All participants were third-year undergraduate medical students at Peking University who had finished 12 pre-clinical subjects, including anatomy, biochemistry, immunology, and microbiology. The participants also had undergone two introductory PBL tutorials and were familiar with the PBL procedures.

PBL procedures

All students underwent eight PBL tutorials during one semester in purpose-built classrooms. Each tutorial group consisted of 12 or 13 students facilitated by one PBL tutor. The group was randomly re-assigned every two tutorials.

For each PBL tutorial, students and their tutor met three times over 2 weeks, for a total of 10 hours. Before each PBL case, students were unaware of the topic and contents. They were introduced to the scenario in the first class and were expected to discuss and ascertain what is known, what is unknown, what should be searched and what should be learned. Afterward, students would search and learn the related information, summarize the notes, submit their homework on their own and browse classmates’ homework, by themselves or with their classmates. In the second class, students would discuss and share information and more scenario would be assigned from tutors where they were requested to repeat the reflective procedures after class. After the third class, they were required to review and summarize all learning issues, and gather all materials and summarize the whole case at the end of the tutorial.

Evaluation of students’ performance in the PBL tutorials

The performance evaluation form was first developed by our experts in assessments of medical education, based on literature review [ 30 , 31 , 32 ] and curriculum reform goal of PBL. According to literature, some evaluation form constructed by five aspects, that is the application of knowledge base, clinical reasoning and decision making skills, self-directed learning, collaborative work and attitude during discussion and professionalism. Some included four aspects, that is group skills, learning skills, reasoning and feedback. Some PBL tutorial assessment included four aspects, that is participation and communication skills, cooperation / team-building skills, comprehension / reasoning skills and knowledge / information gathering skills.

The objectives of PBL in our school included: application basic medical sciences for analysis of disease pathogenesis or treatment, cultivation of self-directed, active and life-long learning, promotion of team working spirit, promotion the expression and communication, development of critical thinking dispositions and skills, and development of appropriate professional attitudes and behaviors.

Therefore, combined the literature and our PBL outcomes, the student performance evaluation form consisted of five domains, which is participation, preparation, communication skills, critical discussion, and teamwork.

Students were evaluated by their tutors immediately after the case learning. Each dimension divides into five levels and accounts for 20 scores. The standard of performance is characterized in Table  1 .

The participation dimension measures one’s passion for learning and the attitude towards PBL. Students are expected to attend every class, to submit their homework, and to read through all the learning materials shared by group members on time.

The preparation dimension measures one’s input on learning. Students are obligated to prepare all learning issues and to examine related information after class and submit homework online.

The communication skills dimension measures the ability to communicate with the group members. Students should express their thoughts precisely and concisely in an appropriate manner.

The critical discussion dimension measures the extent of participation and contribution in the group learning. Students should learn the references actively and critically to make comments using substantial evidence and innovative thinking.

The teamwork dimension measures the participation in group learning and the collaboration with other members. Students should accord sufficient respect to their colleagues and tutor and work together with great enthusiasm.

Group classification

To investigate whether and how CT disposition influences the PBL performance in tutorials, students were divided into several groups. According to the extreme grouping method and the total CT scores, students with total CT scores in the top 27% of all students were defined as Strong-CT Students, and those whose total CT scores were in the bottom 27% were defined as Weak-CT Students.

Overall, eight successive PBL tutorials were conducted in one semester; the first four tutorials were classified as Early-Half PBL tutorials, and the remaining four tutorials were classified as Late-Half PBL tutorials.

Instruments for CT disposition assessment

The critical thinking disposition was usually measured by the California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory (CCTDI) which was first developed by Facione in 1994 [ 22 ]. In 2004, Peng et al. in The Hong Kong Polytechnic University translated and modified CCTDI in consideration of Chinese culture, developed a Chinese version of critical thinking disposition inventory (CTDI-CV) [ 33 ]. After that, most studies in China used this CTDI-CV to assess the critical thinking disposition [ 29 , 34 ]. The CTDI-CV is a standardized, six Likert multiple choice test that consisted of 70 items, which could be divided into seven subscales: truth-seeking, open-mindedness, analyticity, systematicity, CT self-confidence, inquisitiveness, and cognitive maturity. Each subscale had 10 items, and its score ranged from 10 to 60. A total score above 280 and subscale scores above 40 were considered as positive CT disposition. A total score below 210 and subscale scores below 30 were considered as negative CT disposition.

Students were allowed to finish the questionnaire via an internet website, where the data were collected from their submission.

Statistical analysis

This study was quantitatively researched, and the numerical data were analyzed by SPSS 16.0. A P -value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

The general descriptive analysis of CTDI-CV showed students were at a positive CT level

Among a total of 168 students at Peking University, 102 (60.71%) students finished the CTDI-CV test. The CT scores ranged from 236 to 370, with a mean score of 297.72 and a standard deviation of 29.80. By assigning the index score to 280, scores above 280 were defined as a positive attitude, and the scores below 210 were defined as a negative attitude. The result showed that the CT disposition of third-year undergraduate medical students at Peking University was at a positive level on average. An analysis of seven subscale dimensions also indicated that approximately 50% of students were at a positive level (Table  2 ).

A significant positive association was observed between CT dispositions and PBL performance

To investigate whether CT disposition correlates with PBL performance among the students, a parametric bivariate correlation analysis was performed between the students’ CT scores and their eight successive PBL average scores. The results showed that the total CT scores had positive correlations significantly with the scores of the PBL performance and all five dimensions. Among the seven subscales, the scores of open-mindedness, analyticity, CT self-confidence and inquisitiveness showed positive correlations with the PBL total scores. In addition, open-mindedness, analyticity and CT self-confidence showed positive correlations with almost all dimensions of the PBL performance (Table  3 ).

CT disposition plays a crucial role in students’ performance in the PBL process

To investigate whether CT disposition is crucial to students’ performance in the PBL process, the PBL average scores and the scores in the Early-Half or Last-Half PBL were compared between the two extreme CT disposition groups.

First, students with strong CT dispositions obtained higher scores in PBL tutorials

When the eight PBL tutorial average scores were compared between the Strong-CT students and Weak-CT students, significant differences in the total scores and the three dimensions of PBL performance between the two groups were found (Table  4 ).

Second, the performance of these two groups was not different in the early-half PBL but was significantly different in the late-half PBL

It was observed that the average scores of the PBL and the five dimensions in the two groups were not different in the Early-Half PBL tutorials. However, in the Late-Half PBL tutorials, students with strong CT dispositions had higher PBL scores than students with weak CT dispositions (Table  5 ).

Third, a significant improvement was observed in students with strong CT dispositions but not in students with weak CT dispositions

The improvement scores, that is, the average scores of the Late-Half PBL after subtracting the First-Half PBL, were compared between the two extreme CT disposition groups. The average improvement score of the CT-Strong group significantly increased; conversely, the score decreased in students with weak CT dispositions, particularly in the dimensions of preparation, communication skills and critical discussion dimensions (Table  6 ).

An open-mindedness subscale of CT disposition may be the central component in students’ performance

To investigate which subscale of CT disposition is crucial to improving the students’ performance in the PBL process and to execute on the appropriate dimensions of the PBL performance, a stepwise multiple linear regression was conducted by the improvement scores. The five dimensions of the PBL performance were successively used as the dependent variable, and the subscale of the CT disposition was used as the independent variables. It was found that the open-mindedness subscale of CT disposition was the primary factor that influences the improvement of the Preparation dimensions in the PBL process (Table  7 ).

Our results revealed a favorable critical thinking disposition among third-year undergraduate medical students at Peking University because the average mean score on the CTDI-CV was 297.72, which was specified by Facione [ 22 ]. Following several other results from Huang L [ 35 ], students from top medical universities showed a positive attitude towards critical thinking, particularly on the subscales of analyticity and inquisitiveness. However, students showed weaker performance on the subscales of truth-seeking and CT self-confidence, which presumably ascribed to the traditional large-classroom teaching methods and the bashfulness of the Chinese culture.

A significant positive association was observed between the total CT scores and the PBL scores resulting from the correlation analysis, which meant that the CT dispositions of the learners would influence their performance during PBL tutorials.

Among the seven CT subscales, the scores of open-mindedness, analyticity, CT self-confidence, and inquisitiveness showed positive correlations with the PBL total scores, implying that students who score higher in these dispositions would be more active in the PBL process and would thus obtain higher PBL scores. Furthermore, the subscale of open-mindedness showed a positive correlation with three performance dimensions: the participation, the preparation, and the communication skills dimension; CT self-confidence showed a positive correlation with the participation and the Critical discussion dimension. According to Facione, the open-mindedness subscale addresses “ being tolerant of divergent views with sensitivity to the possibility of one’s own bias. ” Thus, students score higher in the open-mindedness disposition would prefer to collect various information, to tune in different perspectives, and to express themselves actively; this concept is demonstrated in the higher scores of the participation, the preparation, and the communication skills dimension. A stronger disposition of CT-confidence allows students to relax and participate in the learning process and discussion, which could be reflected in the higher scores of the participation and the critical discussion dimension.

More data supports our hypothesis that CT disposition plays a crucial role in students’ performance in the PBL process. First, students with strong CT dispositions obtained higher PBL scores than those with weak CT dispositions meaning that stronger CT dispositions bring students to learn more actively and to perform much better. Second, it was observed that in the Late-Half PBL tutorials, the average scores of the PBL and the five dimensions in the Strong-CT group were significantly higher than those in the Weak-CT group. On the contrary, in the Early-Half PBL tutorials, they showed no difference, which indicates that Strong-CT disposition students commit more passion in learning, and the knowledge and skills would improve as long as the process of tutorials. Finally, a significant improvement was observed in students with a strong CT disposition, which suggest that strong CT dispositions stimulate students to advance steadily. However, in the Weak-CT group, the students would behave consistently throughout whole semester without significant improvement.

Overall, our results showed that CT disposition positively correlates to a students’ PBL performance. Students with stronger CT dispositions would perform better in the PBL process and obtain higher scores. The open-mindedness of the CT disposition is the primary factor that determines the improvement of the preparation dimensions in the PBL process. Thus, the cultivation of open-mindedness plays a beneficial role in learning.

However, some limitations of this study include the following aspects. First, the 102 students involved in this study is considered as a limited sample pool. Nonetheless, these 102 students were enrolled with similar admission scores and underwent the same curricular system, which had similar features to reveal the PBL scores and CT dispositions. Second, the positive correlation between PBL scores and CT disposition could be explained by the generosity grading scales conducted by the tutors, leading to all students received similar PBL scores with low or no significance.

Conclusions

Collectively, this study described the details of how CT disposition influence the performance in PBL tutorials. According to the consensus statement regarding critical thinking and the ideal critical thinker, “CT is a liberating force in education and a powerful resource in one’s personal and civic life.” Previous studies were most concentrated on how the CT disposition or its’ influence factor was, or whether the CT disposition was improved after educational reform. Although several studies mentioned that PBL might help students to improve their critical thinking, no studies considered whether and how CT and its subscales influence students’ performance in the PBL process. Our research demonstrated that CT disposition of students might also influence the learning efficiency and learning outcomes in PBL tutorials. Therefore, as a tutor in PBL tutorial or the manager in the medical curriculum, it is crucial to recognize the potential influence of critical thinking dispositions on the outcome of PBL. Systematic guiding or training of students, which would help them to acclimatize to PBL pedagogy and learning methods, should be enhanced throughout the whole course. The tutor training should be improved to promote the teaching and learning effects.

Abbreviations

  • Critical thinking

The Chinese version of the Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory

  • Problem-based learning

Preeti B, Ashish A, Shriram G. Problem based learning (PBL) - an effective approach to improve learning outcomes in medical teaching. J Clin Diagn Res. 2013;7(12):2896–7.

Google Scholar  

Ibrahim ME, Al-Shahrani AM, Abdalla ME, Abubaker IM, Mohamed ME. The effectiveness of problem-based learning in Acquisition of Knowledge, soft skills during basic and preclinical sciences: medical Students’ points of view. Acta Inform Med. 2018;26(2):119–24.

Article   Google Scholar  

Servant-Miklos VFC. Problem solving skills versus knowledge acquisition: the historical dispute that split problem-based learning into two camps. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-018-9835-0 .

Bjork RA, Dunlosky J, Kornell N. Self-regulated learning: beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:417.

Colliver JA. Effectiveness of problem-based learning curricula: research and theory. Acad Med. 2000;75(3):259–66.

Bate E, Hommes J, Duvivier R, Taylor DC. Problem-based learning (PBL): getting the most out of your students - their roles and responsibilities: AMEE guide no. 84. Med Teach. 2014;36(1):1–12.

Schmidt HG, Vermeulen L, van der Molen HT. Longterm effects of problem-based learning: a comparison of competencies acquired by graduates of a problem-based and a conventional medical school. Med Educ. 2006;40(6):562–7.

Schmidt HG, Cohen-Schotanus J, Arends LR. Impact of problem-based, active learning on graduation rates for 10 generations of Dutch medical students. Med Educ. 2009;43(3):211–8.

Wood DF. Problem based learning. Bmj. 2008;336(7651):971.

Dolmans DH, Loyens SM, Marcq H, Gijbels D. Deep and surface learning in problem-based learning: a review of the literature. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2016;21(5):1087–112.

Wood DF. Problem based learning. Bmj. 2003;326(7384):328–30.

Azer SA, Peterson R, Guerrero AP, Edgren G. Twelve tips for constructing problem-based learning cases. Med Teach. 2012;34(5):361–7.

Azer SA. Twelve tips for creating trigger images for problem-based learning cases. Medical Teacher. 2007;29(2–3):93–7.

Basu Roy R, McMahon GT. Video-based cases disrupt deep critical thinking in problem-based learning. Med Educ. 2012;46(4):426–35.

Dammers J, Spencer J, Thomas M. Using real patients in problem-based learning: students’ comments on the value of using real, as opposed to paper cases, in a problem-based learning module in general practice. Med Educ. 2001;35(1):27–34.

Chng E, Yew EH, Schmidt HG. Effects of tutor-related behaviours on the process of problem-based learning. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2011;16(4):491–503.

Chng E, Yew EH, Schmidt HG. To what extent do tutor-related behaviours influence student learning in PBL? Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2015;20(1):5–21.

Visschers-Pleijers AJ, Dolmans DH, de Grave WS, Wolfhagen IH, Jacobs JA, van der Vleuten CP. Student perceptions about the characteristics of an effective discussion during the reporting phase in problem-based learning. Med Educ. 2006;40(9):924–31.

Ahmed Z. Problems of group dynamics in problem based learning sessions. J Ayub Med Coll Abbottabad. 2014;26(2):230–4.

Facione PA. Critical thinking: A Statement of Expert Consensus for Purposes of Educational Assessment and Instruction. Millbrae: The California Academic Press; 1990.

Papp KK, Huang GC, Clabo LML, Delva D, Fischer M, Konopasek L, Schwartzstein RM, Gusic M. Milestones of critical thinking: a developmental model for medicine and nursing. Acad Med. 2014;89(5):715–20.

Facione NC, Facione PA, Sanchez CA. Critical thinking disposition as a measure of competent clinical judgment: the development of the California critical thinking disposition inventory. J Nurs Educ. 1994;33(8):345–50.

Core Committee of the Institute for International Medical Education. Global minimum essential requirements in medical education. Med Teach. 2002;24(2):130–5.

Chang MJ, Chang YJ, Kuo SH, Yang YH, Chou FH. Relationships between critical thinking ability and nursing competence in clinical nurses. J Clin Nurs. 2011;20(21–22):3224–32.

Ghazivakili Z, Norouzi Nia R, Panahi F, Karimi M, Gholsorkhi H, Ahmadi Z. The role of critical thinking skills and learning styles of university students in their academic performance. J Adv Med Educ Prof. 2014;2(3):95–102.

Athari ZS, Sharif SM, Nasr AR, Nematbakhsh M. Assessing critical thinking in medical sciences students in two sequential semesters: does it improve? J Educ Health Promot. 2013;2:5.

Yu D, Zhang Y, Xu Y, Wu J, Wang C. Improvement in critical thinking dispositions of undergraduate nursing students through problem-based learning: a crossover-experimental study. J Nurs Educ. 2013;52(10):574–81.

Ross D, Loeffler K, Schipper S, Vandermeer B, Allan GM. Do scores on three commonly used measures of critical thinking correlate with academic success of health professions trainees? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acad Med. 2013;88(5):724–34.

Hong SH, Yu P. Comparison of the effectiveness of two styles of case-based learning implemented in lectures for developing nursing students’ critical thinking ability: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Nurs Stud. 2017;68:16–24.

Dalrymple KR, Wong S, Rosenblum A, Wuenschell C, Paine M, Shuler CF. PBL Core skills faculty development workshop 3: understanding PBL process assessment and feedback via scenario-based discussions, observation, and role-play. J Dent Educ. 2007;71(12):1561–73.

Sim SM, Azila NMA, Lian LH, Tan CPL, Tan NH. A simple instrument for the assessment of student performance in problem-based learning tutorials. Ann Acad Med Singap. 2006;35(9):634–41.

Valle R, Petra I, Martinez-Gonzalez A, Rojas-Ramirez JA, Morales-Lopez S, Pina-Garza B. Assessment of student performance in problem-based learning tutorial sessions. Med Educ. 1999;33(11):818–22.

Peng M, Wang G, Chen J, Chen M, Bai H, Li S, Li J, Cai Y, Wang J, Yin L. Validity and reliability of the chinese critical thinking disposition inventory. J Chin J Nurs. 2004;39(9):644–7.

Hwang SY, Yen M, Lee BO, Huang MC, Tseng HF. A critical thinking disposition scale for nurses: short form. J Clin Nurs. 2010;19(21–22):3171–6.

Huang L, Wang Z, Yao Y, Shan C, Wang H, Zhu M, Lu Y, Sun P, Zhao X. Exploring the association between parental rearing styles and medical students’ critical thinking disposition in China. BMC Med Educ. 2015;15:88.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all study participants for their contribution to the research.

This work was supported by National Found for Fostering Talents of Basic Science (J1103605/J0108) from National Natural Science Foundation of China, Peking University Health Science Center 2017 Medical Education Research Project (2017JJ10), Chinese Medical Association Medical Education Branch and China Higher Education Society Medical Education Specialized Committee 2016 Medical Education Research Project (2016B-RC021).

The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; the preparation of the manuscript; or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Office of Education, Peking University School of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, 100083, China

Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Peking University School of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, 100083, China

Department of Physiology and Pathophysiology, Peking University School of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, 100083, China

Demao Song, Liling Wu & Xian Wang

Department of Anatomy, Peking University School of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, 100083, China

Weiguang Zhang

Department of Immunology, Peking University School of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, 100083, China

Yuedan Wang

Department of Neurobiology, Peking University School of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, 100083, China

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

PD performed data collection, interpretation, statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. NJH, SDM, ZWG, WYD, WX and WLL performed data interpretation, and helped to draft the manuscript. WY designed this study, contributed to interpret the data and draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yun Wang .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The Research Ethics Board at Peking University Health Science Center approved the study. All participants were conveyed about the study by email and by our local PBL website, written informed consent was obtained once they filled the questionnaire.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the article.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Pu, D., Ni, J., Song, D. et al. Influence of critical thinking disposition on the learning efficiency of problem-based learning in undergraduate medical students. BMC Med Educ 19 , 1 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-018-1418-5

Download citation

Received : 06 October 2018

Accepted : 03 December 2018

Published : 03 January 2019

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-018-1418-5

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Personal characteristics
  • Learning efficiency

BMC Medical Education

ISSN: 1472-6920

critical thinking disposition self rating form

A critical thinking disposition scale for nurses: short form

Affiliation.

  • 1 Department of Nursing, Chung Hwa University of Medical Technology, Tainan, Taiwan.
  • PMID: 21040020
  • DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2010.03343.x

Aims and objectives: The aim of this study was to test the Chinese version of the Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory (CTDI-CV) among nurses in Taiwan.

Background: Critical thinking is the use of purposeful self-regulatory judgments to identify patient's problems and provide patient care. Critical thinking influences nurses' decision making. To date, no inventory to understand nurse's critical thinking disposition has been developed.

Design: This was a survey design with a stratified random sampling to test the reliability and validity of the CTDI-CV.

Methods: The participants comprised 864 registered nurses who were chosen by stratified random sampling from seven hospitals in Taiwan. Data were collected through self-administered structured questionnaires.

Results: A new scale, short form (SF) CTDI-CV, contains 18 items with three subscales: 'systematic analysis', 'thinking within the box' and 'thinking out of the box', was generated from the analysis with 44% explained variance. Cronbach's alpha coefficients and intra-class correlation coefficients for overall and subscale were above 0.8. Goodness-of-fit test for the final model of SF-CTDI-CV revealed an acceptable result in the overall fit (χ(2)/df = 4.04, p < 0.05, GFI = 0.93, AGFI = 0.91, SRMR = 0.076, RMSEA = 0.059).

Conclusion: On the basis of these results, the SF-CTDI-CV is a reliable instrument for assessing critical thinking disposition for nurses.

Relevance to clinical practice: A short and valid critical thinking instrument for nurses will facilitate critical thinking research in the clinical practice arena. When designing continuing education activities, clinical educators will be able to efficiently and effectively evaluate the quality of critical thinking among practicing nurses.

© 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

  • Cross-Sectional Studies
  • Decision Making
  • Employee Performance Appraisal / methods*
  • Factor Analysis, Statistical
  • Middle Aged
  • Nurse Clinicians / standards*
  • Nursing Diagnosis / standards*
  • Practice Patterns, Nurses'
  • Professional Competence*
  • Psychometrics
  • Reproducibility of Results
  • Surveys and Questionnaires

IMAGES

  1. Critical Thinking Disposition Self-Rating Form

    critical thinking disposition self rating form

  2. What can one learn by using the critical thinking disposition self

    critical thinking disposition self rating form

  3. Preliminary psychometric characteristics of the critical thinking self

    critical thinking disposition self rating form

  4. Critical Thinking Disposition Self- Rating Form.

    critical thinking disposition self rating form

  5. Measuring Critical Thinking Disposition Rubric

    critical thinking disposition self rating form

  6. Cottrell_critical thinking skills_SELF_EVALUATION.pdf

    critical thinking disposition self rating form

VIDEO

  1. What is Critical Thinking?

  2. Critical Thinking Part 2: Components and Development

  3. What is Critical Thinking and 7 Reasons Why Critical Thinking is Important

  4. Using the Tools of Critical Thinking for Effective Decision Making

  5. 5 Steps To Better Decision Making

  6. Critical Thinking Skills: A Process for Better Problem Solving and Decision Making

COMMENTS

  1. Critical Thinking Disposition Self-Rating Form

    Critical Thinking Disposition Self-rating Form - Free download as Word Doc (.doc / .docx), PDF File (.pdf), Text File (.txt) or read online for free. Critical Thinking Disposition Self-rating Form

  2. PDF Critical Thinking in Psychology (& Life) Workshop Series: Instructional

    - Critical Thinking Disposition Self-Rating Form (Facione, 2013) Full references and links (when possible) to these measures are listed at the end of the document ... • Critical thinking about psychology in particular is important because psychology is important to life! (e.g. coping with stress, improving relationships, discerning motives)

  3. Critical Thinking Disposition Self- Rating Form.

    even numbered items. If your total is 70 or<br />. above, you are rating your disposition toward<br />. critical thinking over the past two days<br />. as generally positive. Scores of 50 or lower<br />. indicate a self-rating that is averse or hostile<br />. toward critical thinking over the past two<br />. days.

  4. PDF Critical Thinking Self-Study Evaluation Tool: An Introduction

    to the self-study provider in order to receive the provider issued certificate of completion. Critical thinking can be defined as the objective analysis, interpretation and evaluation of a topic or issue in order to form a judgment. Good critical thinking can draw reasonable conclusion from a set of information

  5. Lesson 1.5 Critical Thinking Mindset Self-Rating

    The following activity was adapted from their Critical Thinking Mindset Self-Rating Form. Reflect on your thinking over the last two days and answer the following questions. If you have described yourself honestly, this self-rating form can offer a rough estimate of what you think your overall disposition toward critical thinking has been in ...

  6. 12 Important Dispositions for Critical Thinking

    Critical thinking (CT) consists of a number of skills and dispositions that, when used appropriately, increases the chances of producing a logical solution to a problem or a valid conclusion to an ...

  7. (PDF) Cultivating A Critical Thinking Mindset 1

    Here, we focus on the study of translation, adaptation, and validation of the Critical Thinking Mindset Self-Rating Form ... The California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory (CCTDI ...

  8. How Do Critical Thinking Ability and Critical Thinking Disposition

    Introduction. Although there is no consistent definition of critical thinking (CT), it is usually described as "purposeful, self-regulatory judgment that results in interpretation, analysis, evaluation, and inference, as well as explanations of the evidential, conceptual, methodological, criteriological, or contextual considerations that judgment is based upon" (Facione, 1990, p. 2).

  9. The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking

    disposition toward CT. The California Critical Thinking Disposi. tion Inventory (CCTDI) (Facione and Facione 1992), which. derives its conceptualization of the disposition toward CT from. the APA Delphi Report, is the first such instrument. Building on. the power of a relatively rare occurrence in research, a cross.

  10. The development and psychometric validation of a Critical Thinking

    Abstract. This article describes the development and psychometric evaluation of a Critical Thinking Disposition Scale (CTDS). Items for the scale were generated using taxonomies of important thinking dispositions discussed in the literature. Participants comprised of two cohorts of first year undergraduate and graduate students enrolled on a ...

  11. Cultivating A Critical Thinking Mindset

    The "Critical Thinking Mindset Self-Rating Form" on the next page offers one way of reflecting on our own values, beliefs, and intentions about the application of critical thinking to life's daily problems and decisions. ... The "Critical Thinking Disposition Self-Rating Form," presented earlier, will give us a rough idea. If we have ...

  12. (PDF) The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking: Its Character

    In 1990, a cross-disciplinary international panel of 46 experts came up with a definition of critical thinking after two years of research and deliberation to include the purposeful, self ...

  13. Critical Thinking Dispositions

    Critical thinking (CT) skills and dispositions are increasingly valued in modern society, largely because these skills and dispositions support reasoning and problem-solving in real-world settings ...

  14. (PDF) The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 1

    The California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory (CCTDI), developed in 1992, was used to sample college students at two comprehensive universities. Entering college freshman students showed ...

  15. PDF A gap analysis of critical thinking skills

    A gap analysis of critical thinking skills and attitude toward critical thinking among interns Zayabalaradjane Zayapragassarazan1, Thomas V Chacko2,3 ... skills and with CT disposition self‑rating form[20] containing 20 items to assess their attitude toward CT, respectively. The Hogan Lovells CT appraisal tool is an

  16. What are the Key Dispositions of Good Critical Thinkers?

    Critical thinking (CT) is commonly defined as a metacognitive process, consisting of a number of cognitive skills (e.g. analysis, evaluation and inference) and a variety of personal dispositions ...

  17. Development and Validation of a Critical Thinking Assessment-Scale

    This study presents and validates the psychometric characteristics of a short form of the Critical Thinking Self-assessment Scale (CTSAS). The original CTSAS was composed of six subscales representing the six components of Facione's conceptualisation of critical thinking. The CTSAS short form kept the same structures and reduced the number of items from 115 in the original version, to 60.

  18. PDF The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking: Its Character, Measurement

    The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking: Its Character, Measurement, and Relationship to Critical Thinking Skill ... evaluation, and comparison by one's self and others. Namable and describable ...

  19. Influence of critical thinking disposition on the learning efficiency

    Background Problem-based learning (PBL), a pedagogical approach, is widely accepted in medical education. Manipulated by many factors, the internal motivation of learner is the most crucial determinant that affects the nature of the outcome, in which the influences of critical thinking (CT) remained elusive. Methods One hundred two third-year undergraduate medical students at Peking University ...

  20. PDF Critical Thinking Self-Study Evaluation Tool: An Introduction

    The original is to be submitted to the self-study provider in order to receive the provider issued certificate of completion. Critical thinking can be defined as the objective analysis, interpretation and evaluation of a topic or issue in order to form a judgment. Good critical thinking can draw reasonable conclusion from a set of information

  21. A critical thinking disposition scale for nurses: short form

    Results: A new scale, short form (SF) CTDI-CV, contains 18 items with three subscales: 'systematic analysis', 'thinking within the box' and 'thinking out of the box', was generated from the analysis with 44% explained variance. Cronbach's alpha coefficients and intra-class correlation coefficients for overall and subscale were above 0.8.

  22. PDF Critical Thinking Self-Study Evaluation Tool: An Introduction

    to the self-study provider in order to receive the provider issued certi˚cate of completion. Critical thinking can be de˚ned as the objective analysis, interpretation and evaluation of a topic or issue in order to form a judgment. Good critical thinking can draw reasonable conclusion from a set of information

  23. philosphy 1116 Flashcards

    what can one learn by using the critical thinking disposition self rating form. a rough estimate of one's current overall attitude towards critical thinking. ... evidence used to reach that conclusion. to respond to this question the social scientist will be primarily using which critical thinking skill? ...