ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Considering the self in the link between self-esteem and materialistic values: the moderating role of self-construal.

\r\nYan Zhang,*

  • 1 School of Media and Communication, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 2 Department of Educational Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China

Studies consistently show that materialism might be a strategy people use to cope with low self-esteem. This link might differ among people holding different definitions of the “self” in terms of their relationships with others, however. This research examined the link between self-esteem and materialistic values from the perspective of how people define the self, or their self-construal. In three studies, we explored the moderating role of self-construal in the link between Chinese participants’ self-esteem and materialistic values. Through a self-report survey (Study 1, N = 422), experimental manipulation of self-construal (Study 2, N = 151), and experimental manipulation of both self-esteem and self-construal (Study 3, N = 123), results indicated that self-esteem and self-construal interacted in predicting materialistic values. Specifically, self-esteem negatively predicted materialistic values when interdependent self-construal was low, but not when it was high. We suggest that individuals’ pursuit of materialism under conditions of low self-esteem might depend on how they define the “self.”

Introduction

Many people consider financial and material success to be their life goals, and place great emphasis on owning high-end products and prestigious brands. Researchers have defined materialistic values as “the importance a person places on possessions and their acquisition as a necessary or desirable form of conduct to reach desired end states, including happiness” ( Richins and Dawson, 1992 , p. 307). However, too much emphasis on material possessions has consistently shown negative links with lower long-term well-being (e.g., Kasser, 2003 , 2005 ; Dittmar et al., 2014 ). Therefore, the question of what causes materialism has attracted much research attention. It has been consistently found that self-esteem is a key variable that negatively predicts materialistic values (e.g., Braun and Wicklund, 1989 ; Hanley and Wilhelm, 1992 ; Chang and Arkin, 2002 ; Solberg et al., 2004 ; Chaplin and John, 2007 , 2010 ). However, prior research has not considered variations in how people define the “self,” which might further lead to different strategies in coping with low self-esteem. We propose that the link between self-esteem and materialistic values might vary based on people’s self-construal, or how they define the “self” in terms of their relationships with others ( Markus and Kitayama, 1991 ).

The negative link between self-esteem and materialistic values has been demonstrated in both survey and experimental studies. Self-report surveys have indicated that self-esteem is inversely correlated with people’s general materialistic values, purchasing expensive brands, or valuing of material possessions ( Richins and Dawson, 1992 ; Mick, 1996 ; Chaplin and John, 2010 ; Isaksen and Roper, 2012 ). Through meta-analysis on 15 correlational studies that included self-evaluation as an indicator of well-being, mainly based on Western samples, Dittmar et al. (2014) indicated that materialistic values are negatively linked to self-appraisals. That is, they concluded that higher levels of materialistic values are associated with lower self-evaluations.

At the same time, despite that materialism is embossed within a broader values system that forms due to many factors and over a relatively long time, and might therefore remain relatively stable ( Schwartz, 1992 ), they might also shift occasionally due to internal states and environmental cues (e.g., Bauer et al., 2012 ). Experiments focusing on causal effects have also consistently suggested a relationship between self-esteem and materialism, either by directly showing the influence of self-esteem upon materialistic values, or upon consumption tendencies that relate to materialistic values. Manipulation of low self-esteem through experiences of social ostracism promoted materialism ( Jiang et al., 2015 ), and positive social evaluations about personal traits decreased materialistic values ( Chaplin and John, 2007 ). Additionally, Chang and Arkin (2002) manipulated people’s sense of insecurity, a feeling closely linked with low self-esteem, and found elevated levels of materialism among participants. Sivanathan and Pettit (2010) stimulated participants’ need for self-integrity by giving relatively low cognitive ability feedback, and found higher status consumption tendencies. Furthermore, Sheldon and Kasser (2008) tested the impact of three kinds of threats (existential threat, economic threat, and interpersonal threat) on extrinsic goal orientations. After experiencing any of these threats, individuals reported stronger extrinsic goals for money, appearance, and popularity. The studies conveyed consistent information that a state of low self-esteem might increase materialistic values.

Symbolic self-completion theory ( Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1981 ; Brunstein and Gollwitzer, 1996 ) provides an explanation for the negative effect of self-esteem on materialistic values. The theory suggests that, under the condition of impaired self-identity, individuals will seek things or engage in activities that can help them regain a positive self-image. Individuals might use the signaling function of material possessions to compensate for threatened self-esteem. The theory aligns with many studies demonstrating the direct negative effect of self-esteem on materialistic values, as the financial success and security represented by material possessions might temporarily boost self-esteem (e.g., Chang and Arkin, 2002 ).

However, purchasing materialistic possessions as a means to compensate for low self-esteem might vary between individuals, depending on their “self” constructions. Research has implied that people might define the “self” differently in relation to others, and therefore might seek distinct means of fulfilling self-esteem. In other words, peoples’ self-construal might moderate the link between self-esteem and materialistic values.

Individuals with an independent self-construal more strongly emphasize uniqueness and personal achievement, while people with an interdependent self-construal focus more strongly on social connectedness and relations ( Markus and Kitayama, 1991 ). Although some researchers have suggested that everyone possesses both aspects self-construal (e.g., Singelis, 1994 ), it is also agreed that people typically prioritize one over the other ( Markus and Kitayama, 1991 ), and that being high in one type of construal often implies being low in the other. When individuals focus on pursuing self-expression and personal uniqueness, they are less likely to be relationship-oriented or emphasize connectedness with others ( Markus and Kitayama, 1991 ). Moreover, individuals might also shift to a corresponding independent or interdependent self-construal, respectively, under priming conditions such as searching for words like “I” and “mine” or “we” and “ours” in paragraphs (e.g., Trafimow et al., 1991 ; Brewer and Gardner, 1996 ; Gardner et al., 1999 ; Lin et al., 2008 ). The contrary effects of the two self-construal primes in these former studies further imply that low interdependent self-construal might suggest independent self-construal (for different opinions, see Hardin et al., 2004 ; Harb and Smith, 2008 ). Considering that all the three studies were conducted under Chinese culture, a context where interdependence is emphasized and found to be relatively high (e.g., Li et al., 2006 ), the paper follows precedents set by other studies implemented in collectivistic cultures (e.g., Fung et al., 2010 ) by using the terms high and low interdependent self-construal. This terminology is also in line with what the current research wants to communicate, which is that high interdependent self-construal can buffer the relationship between low self-esteem and materialism in collectivistic cultures.

The different nature of the “self” between individuals with high vs. low interdependent self-construal implies different attitudes toward materialistic values when self-esteem is low. Those who emphasize low interdependence enhance and maintain self-esteem through personal success, fulfillment of personal desires, and the validation of their abilities and unique inner attributes. In contrast, the bases of self-esteem for highly interdependent individuals are group harmony and associations with others ( Markus and Kitayama, 1991 ). This distinction might lead individuals low in interdependence be more prone to seek chances for self-expression. Material possessions and prestigious brands might provide a useful and immediate way to convey information about success, status, and personal ability ( Eastman et al., 1999 ; Sivanathan and Pettit, 2010 ). Therefore, those low in interdependent self-construal might gravitate more toward materialistic values for self-esteem maintenance than individuals high in interdependence.

This proposition is also based on the notion that materialism is indicative of self-centeredness, in line with former research stating that materialistic values were located at the opposite side with collective-orientated values such as family values and religious values in the value system (e.g., Belk, 1985 ; Schwartz, 1992 ; Jensen and Jensen, 1993 ; Kasser and Ryan, 1993 ; La Barbera and Gürhan, 1997 ; Rindfleisch et al., 1997 ; Kasser, 2016 ). The tension caused by individuals’ materialistic values and the collective-oriented values encouraged by the society contributed an undermined well-being in the long run ( Burroughs and Rindfleisch, 2002 ). The conflict between the two types or values aligns with findings suggesting that collectivism, an overt cultural phenomenon of high interdependent self-construal, is negatively correlated with materialism ( Wong, 1997 ). That is, a stronger emphasis on close relationships and group harmony might be linked to a lower emphasis on materialistic values. Another study reported a negative correlation among Chinese participants between interpersonal harmony and regarding material success as a central goal of life ( Sun et al., 2014 ), further supporting the negative association between collective emphasis and materialism. At the same time, studies have shown that priming concepts of money lead to exchange relationship orientation and emotion suppression in social interactions ( Jiang et al., 2013 ), objectification (i.e., treating others as objects; Teng et al., 2016 ; Wang and Krumhuber, 2017 ), and decreased interpersonal harmony ( Mead et al., 2013 ). Such findings imply that materialistic values actually might not be compatible with high quality and close interpersonal relationships. Therefore, those who define the “self” as interdependent and emphasize interpersonal connection are more likely to devalue the importance of material possessions.

The fact that individuals with higher interdependent self-construal emphasize interpersonal connection more, and that materialistic values are more self-oriented, together might contribute to the possibility that individuals differ in employing materialistic values as a coping strategy for low self-esteem. Under self-threat conditions, those who endorse interdependence more would probably be less likely to indicate their uniqueness, while those whose self-definition is less interdependent might express their “self” through material possessions. Indeed, an integral analysis and comparison between lower-interdependent individuals (i.e., Westerners) and higher-interdependent individuals (i.e., Easterners) concluded that highly interdependent individuals experience more social motives (e.g., conformity) but not individually oriented motives (e.g., expressing personal uniqueness) ( Bond, 1986 ; Markus and Kitayama, 1991 ). A meta-analysis from Dittmar et al. (2014) also indicated that the negative link between materialistic values and self-appraisal, was stronger in societies with higher affective autonomy, a typical indicator in the context of low interdependence. Therefore, individuals who more strongly emphasize interrelationships in defining the “self” (i.e., high interdependent self-construal) might be less likely to become materialistic when experiencing low self-esteem.

According to the reviewed evidence, we expect that the negative link between self-esteem and materialistic values will be stronger for individuals scoring lower in interdependent self-construal than for individuals scoring higher in interdependent self-construal. Our use of participants from mainland China led us to focus primarily on low versus high levels of interdependent self-construal, similar to prior research (e.g., Fung et al., 2010 ), as this dimension is highly emphasized in the Chinese context. We implemented three studies: In Study 1, we surveyed self-esteem, interdependent self-construal, and materialistic values, using validated self-report scales. In Study 2, we measured self-esteem and manipulated high- vs. low interdependent self-construal to examine the moderating effect of self-construal. In Study 3, we simultaneously manipulated both self-esteem and self-construal in order to examine whether self-construal and self-esteem exert combined causal effects upon materialistic values.

In this study, using a self-report survey, we expected that a negative link between chronic self-esteem and materialistic values would be stronger for individuals scoring lower in interdependent self-construal.

Participants

Participants were 422 late-adolescents (60.4% female; M age = 17.40 ± 0.86) from a senior high school in a relatively less developed, small county in a medium-sized city in the middle of Mainland China. Prior sample size estimation via G ∗ power indicated a minimum of 176 participants (small to medium effect size f 2 = 0.10, power = 0.95), and sensitivity power analysis indicated 95% power to detect an effect size of f 2 = 0.04 (small) with the actual sample size in this survey. Family socioeconomic status (SES; i.e., yearly income) was measured on a 9-point scale, with the following distribution: 1 = less than 3,000 yuan (2.6%), 2 = 3,001–6,000 yuan (6.4%), 3 = 6,001–10,000 yuan (17.3%), 4 = 10,001–30,000 yuan (29.6%), 5 = 30,001–50,000 yuan (17.8%), 6 = 50,001–100,000 yuan (17.3%), 7 = 100,001–150,000 yuan (4.5%), 8 = 150,001–200,000 yuan (3.6%), and 9 = more than 200,001 yuan (0.9%). The mean score of SES was 4.47 ( SD = 1.59).

Self-esteem

The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES: Rosenberg, 1965 ) measured self-esteem in this study. It was comprised of 10 items answered on a four-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree , 2 = agree , 3 = disagree , and 4 = strongly disagree ). An example item is “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.” We recoded the scores so that higher scores indicated higher self-esteem. Its Cronbach’s alpha was 0.76 in this study.

Interdependent self-construal

We used the interdependent subscale of the Self-Construal Scale (SCS) developed by Singelis (1994) to measure interdependent self-construal, which contains 12 items on a single dimension. An example item is: “I often have the feeling that my relationships with others are more important than my own accomplishments.” Participants answered the items on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree , 7 = strongly agree ). The Cronbach’s alpha in this study was 0.71.

Materialistic values

Materialistic values in this study were measured by the instrument modified from a six-item scale developed by Opree et al. (2011) . The expression of “ children ” in the original items was changed into “ peers .” On a four-point scale (1 = no, not at all , 2 = no, not really , 3 = yes, a little , and 4 = yes, very much ), participants responded to items such as “Do you think it is important to own expensive things?” and “Does buying expensive things make you happy?” The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.82 in this study.

Demographic variables

Demographic information included age, gender, and socioeconomic status.

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Guidelines for Survey and Behavioral Research Ethics, approved by the ethics committee of both the researchers’ home university (Survey and Behavioral Research Ethics of the Chinese University of Hong Kong) and the institutes where they were conducted (the Ethics Committee of Beijing Normal University and the Ethics Committee of Guangxi University), with written informed consent from all subjects (all were older than 16 years old). All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Pencil-and-paper instruments were handed out to the participants during after-school self-study time in classrooms, with the instruments organized in the order described above. Oral instructions were given on the aim and the answering method of the study. All participants finished the scales anonymously.

Data Analysis

We tested the hypotheses via hierarchical multiple regression. We first standardized the scores of age, SES, self-esteem, interdependent self-construal, and materialistic values, and then calculated the product of self-esteem and interdependent self-construal (SE × SC) for testing the interaction. Gender was coded as 0 = female and 1 = male. In the three-step regression, age, gender, and SES were first entered as control variables, with self-esteem and interdependent self-construal entered in the second step, and the product of self-esteem and interdependent self-construal entered in the third step.

The first regression step including age, gender, and SES significantly predicted materialistic values, F (3, 418) = 2.92, p = 0.034, R = 0.14, adjusted R 2 = 0.01. This was due to a significant effect of SES (β = 0.10, t = 2.06, p = 0.040), with higher SES linked to greater materialism. The second step, which added self-esteem and interdependent self-construal, did not significantly improve the model, F (5, 416) = 2.29, p = 0.045, R = 0.16, adjusted R 2 = 0.02; Δ R 2 = 0.02, Δ F (2, 416) = 1.34, p > 0.250. Neither self-esteem (β = −0.05, t = −0.94, p > 0.250) or self-construal (β = −0.06, t = −1.15, p > 0.250) significantly predicted materialistic values. The third step adding the product of self-esteem and self-construal significantly improved the model, F (6, 415) = 3.28, p = 0.004, R = 0.21, adjusted R 2 = 0.03; Δ R 2 = 0.02, Δ F (1, 415) = 8.03, p = 0.005, f 2 = 0.04, power = 0.94. Although neither self-esteem (β = −0.04, t = −0.89, p > 0.250) or self-construal (β = −0.04, t = −0.76, p > 0.250) showed a significant main effect, the interaction [β = 0.14, t = 2.83, p = 0.005, 95% CI for B = (0.03–0.16)] significantly predicted materialistic values (see Table 1 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Regression model of predictors for material values in Study 1.

We further used a simple slopes analysis to disentangle the interaction between self-esteem and interdependent self-construal ( Aiken and West, 1991 ). Low and high values in the simple slopes test for self-esteem and interdependent self-construal were set as one standard deviation above and below the mean. As can be seen in Figure 1 , there was no relationship between self-esteem and materialistic values when interdependent self-construal was high (β = 0.07, t = 1.55, p = 0.123). When interdependent self-construal was low, however, the relationship was negative and significant (β = −0.13, t = 2.94, p = 0.003). The results indicated that, after controlling for age, gender, and SES, self-esteem significantly predicted materialistic values for individuals scoring lower in interdependent self-construal, but not for those scoring higher in interdependent self-construal. These findings were based on chronic measures; the moderating role of self-construal would be further supported if priming a lower versus higher interdependent mindset would again alter the nature of the link between self-esteem and materialism. This was the goal of Study 2.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Moderating effect of interdependent self-construal on the relationship between self-esteem and materialistic values in Study 1.

In Study 2, we manipulated lower vs. higher interdependent self-construal, and examined whether this manipulation moderated a link between participants’ self-reported self-esteem and materialism. We expected that the link between self-esteem and materialistic values would be stronger when lower interdependent self-construal was primed.

Participants were 162 college students from mainland China. Five of them skipped too many items (over half), and six provided irregular answers (e.g., marking the maximum or minimum option on all Likert scale items or reporting obvious contradict answers), and were therefore excluded. In the end, 151 participants were included (60.90% female; M age = 18.92 ± 1.02), with 77 and 74 belonging to the low and high interdependent self-construal conditions, respectively. Prior sample size estimation via G ∗ power indicated a minimum of 119 participants in total ( f 2 = 0.15, power = 0.95), and sensitivity power analysis indicated 95% power to detect an effect size of medium effect size f 2 = 0.12 with the actual sample size in this experiment. The distribution of the participants’ family SES was as follows: 1 = less than 3,000 yuan (6.0%), 2 = 3,001–6,000 yuan (8.6%), 3 = 6,001–10,000 yuan (15.2%), 4 = 10,001–30,000 yuan (17.9%), 5 = 30,001–50,000 yuan (15.9%), 6 = 50,001–100,000 yuan (17.2%), 7 = 100,001–150,001 yuan (11.3%), 8 = 150,001–200,000 yuan (4.0%), and 9 = more than 200,001 yuan (4.0%). The mean score of SES was 4.70 ( SD = 2.02).

The RSES( Rosenberg, 1965 ) measured self-esteem in this study, as in Study 1. Its Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83 in this study.

A four-item, unidimensional scale modified from Kasser (2005) measured materialistic values on a seven-point scale (1 = strongly disagree , 7 = strongly agree ). This scale was developed from several existing materialism scales from Richins and Dawson (1992) , Cohen and Cohen (1996) , Kasser and Ryan (1996) , and Schor (2003, Unpublished), and was believed to reflect the most important components in existing instruments ( Kasser, 2005 ). The unsuitable phrase of “ when I grow up ” for college students in items like “ When I grow up, I want to own lots of money ” was removed. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.62 in the current study.

Demographic information included age, gender, and SES. SES was measured by family income of the past year by the same scale as in Study 1.

Participants completed pencil and paper questionnaires in class. They first reported their self-esteem, and moved on to the high- or low interdependent self-construal priming task.

The priming task was reading essays and answering questions used in prior studies ( Gardner et al., 1999 ; Sui and Han, 2007 ). In each condition, participants read two stories describing a trip to the countryside, with each of the two-paragraph essays containing high interdependent pronouns (e.g., we, us, and ours ) or low interdependent pronouns (e.g., I, me, and mine ). The essays described what “I”/“we” did, what “I”/“we” saw, and other stories happened during the trip. All the contents of the essays were the same between the two conditions except the pronouns. Participants were required to carefully read the essay, count the total number of pronouns, and circle them in the essay.

Participants then completed the materialistic values scale and demographic information. All participants received the high or low interdependent self-construal priming material randomly, and data comparison showed no difference between the two groups on age, gender distribution, SES, or self-esteem level.

We tested the hypotheses via a hierarchical multiple regression procedure. We first standardized the scores of age, SES, and self-esteem. We dummy coded self-construal as −1 (low interdependent self-construal) and +1 (high interdependent self-construal) and then calculated the product of self-esteem and interdependent self-construal. Gender was coded as 0 = female and 1 = male. In the three-step regression, age, gender, and SES were first entered as control variables, with self-esteem and interdependent self-construal entered in the second step, and the product of self-esteem and self-construal entered in the third step.

The first regression step including age, gender, and SES was not significant, F (3, 147) = 0.37, p > 0.250, R = 0.10, R 2 = 0.01. In the second step, the addition of self-esteem and interdependent self-construal significantly improved the model (Δ R 2 = 0.05, Δ F (2, 145) = 3.88, p = 0.023), although the model was still not significant, F (5, 145) = 1.79, p = 0.120. Self-esteem significantly predicted materialistic values (β = −0.22, t = −2.69, p = 0.008), but self-construal did not (β = −0.06, t = −0.77, p > 0.250). In the third step, the interaction of self-esteem and self-construal (SE × SC) was added to the model. Results indicated that the model became significant, and also showed a trend toward significant improvement over the second step [ F (6, 144) = 2.12, p = 0.054, R = 0.29, adjusted R 2 = 0.04, Δ R 2 = 0.02, p = 0.058, f 2 = 0.08, power = 0.83]. The interaction of self-esteem and interdependent self-construal showed a trend toward predicting materialistic values, β = 0.15, t = 1.91, p = 0.058 (see Table 2 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Regression model of predictors for material values in Study 2.

We further used a simple slopes analysis ( Aiken and West, 1991 ) to clarify the nature of the interaction. Low and high values in the simple slopes test were set as one standard deviation above and below the mean. As seen in Figure 2 , when high interdependent self-construal was primed, there was no relationship between self-esteem and materialistic values (β = −0.07, t = −0.58, p > 0.250). When low interdependent self-construal was primed, however, the relationship was negative and significant (β = −0.39, t = −3.36, p = 0.001). Thus, the negative relationship between self-esteem and materialistic values under conditions of low interdependent self-construal was replicated. We then conducted a final experiment to examine the combined causal effects from both self-esteem and interdependent self-construal upon materialistic values.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Moderating effect of interdependent self-construal on the relationship between self-esteem and materialistic values in Study 2.

In Study 3, we employed a bogus “Image Personality Test” to manipulate self-esteem and self-construal simultaneously, and test their combined effects on materialistic values. We again predicted that a combination of low self-esteem and low interdependent self-construal would yield the highest scores of materialistic values.

Participants were 123 college students (73.8% female, one missing; M age = 21.40 ± 2.52, three missing) from mainland China. Prior sample size estimation via G ∗ power indicated a minimum of 119 participants in total (medium effect size f = 0.26, power = 0.80), and sensitivity power analysis indicated 80% power to detect an effect size of f = 0.25 with the actual sample size in this experiment. They were randomly assigned to the high self-esteem/high interdependent self-construal group (HSE-HSC; n = 28), the high self-esteem/low interdependent self-construal group (HSE-LSC; n = 31), the low self-esteem/high interdependent self-construal group (LSE-HSC; n = 36), and the low self-esteem/low interdependent self-construal group (LSE-LSC; n = 28). Their family SES (one missing) was distributed as follows: 1 = less than 5,000 yuan (0%), 2 = 5,001–10,000 yuan (4.9%), 3 = 10,001–30,000 yuan (10.6%), 4 = 30,001–50,000 yuan (17.1%), 5 = 50,001–100,000 yuan (25.2%), 6 = 100,001–150,000 yuan (22.0%), 7 = 150,001–200,000 yuan (8.1%), 8 = 200,001–300,000 yuan (7.3%), 9 = 300,001–500,000 yuan (2.4%), and 10 = more than 500,001 yuan (1.6%). The mean score of SES was 5.25 ( SD = 1.74).

Demographic information included age, gender, SES (family income of the past year, see the participants section).

Manipulation check

We included a single-item scale modified from Aron et al. (1992) to assess the manipulation of self-construal. Participants were required to circle the picture which best described their relationship with others. In our study, we used social groups as the object for relationship consideration, with higher scores indicating higher interdependence. The instruction was: If circle A represents your members of your social groups and circle B represents your “self,” how connected do you feel to members of your social groups at the moment? (see Figure 3 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Figures for self-esteem manipulation check in pilot study of Study 3, and for self-construal manipulation check in formal Study 3.

A similar scale was employed for self-esteem manipulation check in a pilot study before the formal experiment (but was not used in the formal experiment). We asked participants to choose the picture that best described the distance between their current and ideal “self” with the instruction: If circle A represents your “current self” and circle B represents your “ideal self,” how would you describe their relationship at the moment? In our study, higher self-esteem scores were given (from 1 to 7) when participants selected a picture with a greater overlap between their current and ideal selves ( Figure 3 ). We separated the self-esteem manipulation check in a pilot study to avoid confusion in understanding with the self-construal manipulation check in formal experiment.

The same four-item unidimensional scale as in Study 2, modified from Kasser (2005) measured materialistic values on a seven-point scale (1 = strongly disagree , 7 = strongly agree ). The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.65 in the current study.

Participants finished the task in lab in both the pilot study and the formal study in Study 3. We created a bogus “Image-Personality Test” in order to manipulate the independent variables in this study. Before the formal experiment, we conducted a pilot study to check the efficacy of the program in manipulating self-esteem.

In the pilot study, we recruited a total of 177 participants ( M age = 19.35 ± 1.16, 63.8% female), who were randomly designated into high ( n = 91) or low ( n = 86) self-esteem conditions, with no difference on age, gender, and SES between the groups. They first completed the bogus “Image-Personality Test.” in which they were required to choose one out of four pictures composed of different colors, lines, and patterns, but also shared some common characteristics in every trial and therefore could be viewed as ostensibly being linked to certain personality traits. After a total of 24 trials, participants received specific feedback for each of five personality traits (including conscientiousness, agreeableness, openness, self-control, and flexibility), with a bogus mean score of all participants also presented for reference.

Self-esteem was manipulated by giving feedback showing either high scores (high SE) or low scores (low SE) relative to the “average” score of other participants. The feedback for the high self-esteem condition was as follows, with a figure presented at the same time ( Figure 4 ):

“In this test, each of the five traits was reflected on 6 items, and the score varied from 0 to 6. According to the test on your traits just now, you generally received high scores (with the mean score higher than 78% participants in our experiment, on average) on the five aspects we measured. Below is your score on each aspect.”

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Feedback for bogus “Image Personality Test” in the high self-esteem condition (Study 3).

The feedback for low self-esteem condition was ( Figure 5 ):

“ In this test, each of the five traits was reflected on 6 items, and the score varied from 0 to 6. According to the test on your traits just now, you generally received low scores (with the mean score lower than 78% participants, or only higher than 22% others in our experiment, on average) on the five aspects we measured. Below is your score on each aspect .”

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Feedback for bogus “Image Personality Test” in the low self-esteem condition (Study 3).

After the participants read the feedback, they answered the question for the manipulation check on self-esteem as described in measures section. After controlling age, gender, and SES, a t test in SPSS indicated that the high self-esteem group ( M = 3.78 ± 1.43) and low self-esteem group ( M = 3.29 ± 1.42) reported significantly different distances between their current and ideal selves, t (175) = 2.30, p = 0.023, 95% CI for the mean differences was [0.07 – 0.92] without zero included, suggesting that the “Image-Personality Test” was effective in manipulating self-esteem and therefore could be used in formal experiment.

In the formal study, we further added the manipulation of self-construal in the test. An initial cover story was given explaining that the study was composed of tasks from several unrelated projects, and the whole study contained two parts: the first part was a newly developed “image-personality test” (the manipulations of self-esteem and self-construal), and the second part was a survey on college students’ lifestyles. After providing demographic information, participants were designated randomly into one of the four manipulation groups: HSE-HSC, HSE-LSC, LSE-HSC, and LSE-LSC. Therefore, this was a 2 (self-esteem: high vs. low) × 2 (interdependent self-construal: high vs. low) factorial design. No significant differences on gender distribution, age, or SES were found between the four groups. The participants first completed the bogus “Image-Personality Test” as they did in our pilot study. In the feedback stage of the test, participants received the specific feedback on their scores on each of the five traits used in the pilot study, and additionally received feedback on the extent to which they might share the personality characteristics with other members of their social groups.

Interconnectedness to others ( Aron et al., 1992 ) is important for individuals with high interdependent self-construal and similarity produces perceptions of closeness ( Liviatan et al., 2008 ; Boot and Pecher, 2010 ). Evidence from research on interpersonal behavior has supported the associations between similarity and interdependent self-construal. For example, non-conscious behavioral mimicry, a signal of interpersonal similarity, actually results in participants’ shifts toward more interdependent self-construal ( Van Baaren et al., 2003 ; Ashton-James et al., 2007 ). Therefore, a high similarity feedback could arouse perceptions of interconnectedness and prime an interdependent mindset.

The feedback paragraph for high interdependent self-construal group was as follows (see also Figure 6 ):

“ The pattern of your answers suggests a very high likelihood (up to 82%) that many members of the social groups that you are in would receive scores that are similar to your own, namely that you and they share the same tendencies on these personality traits. Two circles below reflect the patterns, and the overlapping part indicates the shared personality characteristics between you and your group members .”

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Feedback for bogus “Image Personality Test” in the high interdependent self-construal condition (Study 3).

The feedback for low interdependent group was as follows (see also Figure 7 ):

“ The pattern of your answers suggests a very low likelihood (18% or less) that members of the social groups that you are in would receive scores that are similar to your own, namely that you are quite different from them on these personality traits. Two circles below reflect the patterns, and the small overlap indicates that you only shared very limited personality characteristics with your group members .”

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Feedback for bogus “Image Personality Test” in the low interdependent self-construal condition (Study 3).

After the participants received the feedback, they answered questions for the manipulation check of self-construal, and then the self-report items for materialistic values. After the task, they were asked for their opinions about the aim of the study, thanked with a reward, and debriefed.

Data analysis was conducted in SPSS. We first dummy coded the conditions of high- and low self-esteem as +1 and −1, and the conditions of high- and low- interdependent self-construal as +1 and −1. Gender was coded as 0 = female and 1 = male. We then checked that there was no difference on gender distribution, SES, and age between the four groups. We used a two-way ANOVA to check the main effects of self-esteem and self-construal, and their interaction on materialistic values.

Manipulation Check of Self-Construal

Manipulation check on self-construal was conducted with a t test. It indicated that the high interdependent self-construal group ( M = 4.45 ± 1.46) and low interdependent self-construal group ( M = 3.69 ± 1.45) reported significantly different self-construal, t (121) = 2.89, p = 0.005, 95% CI for mean difference = (0.24–1.28), suggesting that the added feedback manipulation of self-construal was effective.

Interaction Effect Between Self-Esteem and Self-Construal

We used a two-way ANOVA to test the main effects and the interaction of self-esteem and self-construal manipulations on materialistic values, with age, gender, and SES entered as covariates. Neither the self-esteem [ F (1,113) = 2.16, p = 0.145, η p 2 = 0.02], or the interdependent self-construal [ F (1,113) = 1.11, p = 0.295, η p 2 = 0.01] manipulations showed direct effects on materialistic values. However, the interaction between self-esteem and self-construal showed a significant effect on materialistic values [ F (1,113) = 4.48, p = 0.036, η p 2 = 0.04, power = 0.64].

A simple effects test was used to examine specific differences in materialistic values between the four experimental conditions. It indicated that low self-esteem priming ( M lse–hsc = 4.99 ± 0.97) and high self-esteem priming ( M hse–hsc = 5.07 ± 1.24) did not lead to significantly different materialistic values within the high interdependent self-construal condition, F (1, 113) = 0.21, p = 0.647, η p 2 = 0.002. However, higher materialistic values were observed for the low self-esteem condition ( M lse–lsc = 5.59 ± 0.77) than for the high self-esteem condition ( M hse–lsc = 4.82 ± 1.01) when low interdependent self-construal was primed, F (1, 113) = 6.22, p = 0.014, η p 2 = 0.05, power = 0.72. Levels of materialistic values of the four groups are presented in Figure 8 .

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Levels of materialistic values under each condition in Study 3. ∗ p < 0.05.

Our simultaneous manipulation of both self-esteem and self-construal further demonstrated their combined causal effects upon materialistic values. We found that priming low self-esteem increased materialistic values for the individuals who were primed with low interdependent self-construal, but there was no effect of self-esteem on materialistic values for the participants primed with high interdependent self-construal.

General Discussion

In three studies, we found that people’s higher or lower interdependent self-construal, both in terms of chronic states and temporary priming conditions, moderated their emphasis on materialistic values when experiencing self-threat. This research challenges the conventional view that low self-esteem is a stable mechanism for high materialistic values, by showing that the link only holds for individuals with lower levels of interdependent self-construal.

Symbolic self-completion theory ( Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1981 ) can explain people’s materialistic behaviors under conditions of low self-regard. Our research, however, demonstrated that the effect of self-esteem on materialistic values might further depend on how people define the self. Materialism might be a way to fulfill individuals’ goals for uniqueness and validation of personal attributes. Therefore, when experiencing low self-esteem, their valuation of material possessions would be elevated. For the individuals who define the “self” more in terms of relationships with their social contexts and close others, however, intense self-expression through material possessions might not be the most efficient way to maintain self-esteem. This distinction implies that, although all people are motivated to maintain self-esteem ( Leary and Baumeister, 2000 ), they might enact different strategies depending on where their sense of “self” comes from. Prestigious brands might not be sufficient to buffer low self-esteem for those who strongly emphasize their interconnections with others. This appears to be the case regardless of whether self-esteem and self-construal are chronically measured or experimentally manipulated.

Despite that the current research demonstrated a buffering effect of high interdependent self-construal to low self-esteem individuals’ materialism, the relatively high level of materialistic values in the collectivistic Chinese society calls for more explanation. While a higher interdependent self-construal would protect individuals from materialism under conditions of low self-esteem, the generally high materialistic values of a society could be a result of many factors. As a rapidly developing country, China has experienced dramatically increasing economic power in recent years. On a national level, this could lead to value transformations toward materialism ( Inglehart et al., 1998 ; cf. Zhou, 2009 ). Other factors such as increased marketing efforts across the country and emphasis on hierarchical status in the culture might also contribute to higher materialistic norms and values ( Sangkhawasi and Johri, 2007 ; Goldsmith and Clark, 2012 ).

The present research offers several novel contributions. Theoretically, we showed that low self-esteem does not hold an absolute relationship with high materialistic values, as implied by prior studies (e.g., Richins and Dawson, 1992 ; Chang and Arkin, 2002 ; Chaplin and John, 2007 ). Methodologically, we used a new method of feedback to simultaneously manipulate both self-esteem and self-construal in Study 3. The method we created showed advantages of (1) being able to combine or embed the manipulations of multiple variables, and (2) directly present participants’ attributes in terms of sharing similarities with others, and therefore could be useful for future studies. This research also holds practical implications, especially for interventions aimed at reducing materialistic values. Specifically, as individual holding lower interdependent self-construal are prone to use materialism to cope with threats to the “self” and regain positive self-perception, other methods of providing space for self-expression and demonstrating uniqueness to promote self-esteem might be effective, and would hopefully reduce their materialism. At the same time, emphasis on interconnectedness with others when facing self-threats might also buffer the tendency to adopt materialism. Considering that materialism could potentially impact people’s long-term life satisfaction ( Kasser, 2005 ), educators, practitioners, and parents might consider characteristics of the “self” when planning and implementing interventions aimed at decreasing materialistic values.

Our studies were conducted in three different cities in mainland China that differ in economic levels and local micro-cultures. Participants also spanned from high school students to university students, and varied in their family SES. We also altered our study methods, from self-report questionnaires to a 2 × 2, fully experimental design. These variations in samples and methods, but not in our primary finding, speak to the robustness of these results.

Nevertheless, there are limitations that can be improved in future research. Firstly, the results were based on Chinese participants, who are embedded in a relatively high interdependent cultural context and might hold relatively higher interdependent self-construal, as a whole. Although we manipulated self-construal in Studies 2 and 3, it is possible that people’s default mode of self-construal might still play a role in the process. Therefore, more research is needed to replicate the findings cross-culturally. Secondly, the sample sizes in Study 2 and 3 were relatively small, and were not balanced between groups after excluding those reporting suspicions about the study and manipulations. Including more participants and working to ensure a more balanced distribution between experimental conditions would contribute to further confidence in those results. Thirdly, the mechanisms between self-esteem and materialistic values need more exploration. Individuals showing lower interdependence emphasize autonomy and personal goals, and are more likely to show self-protective and self-enhancement tendencies following personal failures ( Brockner and Chen, 1996 ). Therefore, an underlying self-enhancement process might be further examined. Highly interdependent individuals’ extra emphasis on social connection, including interpersonal motivations such as belongingness needs or collective self-esteem, might factor more heavily into their materialism. Interpersonal influences, such as conformity in consumer decision-making, might therefore be a greater source of materialism for individuals in collectivist cultures ( Hui and Triandis, 1986 ; Choi and Geistfeld, 2004 ). In conclusion, our research indicated that the link between materialistic values and self-esteem might be dependent on how individuals define the “self.”

Author Contributions

YZ contributed substantially to the study concept development, design of the study, analysis and interpretation of the data of the study, and also drafted the work for important intellectual content. SH made substantial contributions to design of the study, the analysis and interpretation the data of the study, and also drafted and revised the work for important intellectual content.

This study was financially supported by both the Ministry-School Cooperation funds (No. 85817) for School of Media and Communication of Shenzhen University, and the project “Morality in the Culture of Materialism and Its Mechanism” (No. 19YJC190029) granted by the Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China. This study also received department support for Full-Time Research Students in Participating Academic Activities from the Department of Educational Psychology at the Chinese University of Hong Kong.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Yan Xu from the Beijing Normal University, Dr. Shuyue Zhang from the Guangxi University, and Mr. Rui Wen from Xinfeng No. 2 Middle School of Ganzhou, China, for their enthusiastic help in data collection.

Aiken, L. S., and West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., and Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596–612. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.63.4.596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashton-James, C., Van Baaren, R. B., Chartrand, T. L., Decety, J., and Karremans, J. (2007). Mimicry and me: the impact of mimicry on self–construal. Soc. Cogn. 25, 518–535. doi: 10.1521/soco.2007.25.4.518

Bauer, M. A., Wilkie, J. E., Kim, J. K., and Bodenhausen, G. V. (2012). Cuing consumerism: situational materialism undermines personal and social well-being. Psychol. Sci. 23, 517–523. doi: 10.1177/0956797611429579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belk, R. W. (1985). Materialism: trait aspects of living in the material world. J. Consum. Res. 12, 265–280.

Bond, M. H. (1986). The Psychology of the Chinese People. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Boot, I., and Pecher, D. (2010). Similarity is closeness: metaphorical mapping in a conceptual task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 942–954. doi: 10.1080/17470210903134351

Braun, O. L., and Wicklund, R. A. (1989). Psychological antecedents of conspicuous consumption. J. Econ. Psychol. 10, 161–187. doi: 10.1016/0167-4870(89)90018-4

Brewer, M. B., and Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this” We”? Levels of collective identity and self representations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 71, 83–93. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.1.83

Brockner, J., and Chen, Y. R. (1996). The moderating roles of self-esteem and self-construal in reaction to a threat to the self: evidence from the People’s Republic of China and the United States. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 71, 603–615. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.3.603

Brunstein, J. C., and Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). Effects of failure on subsequent performance: the importance of self-defining goals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 70, 395–407. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.2.395

Burroughs, J. E., and Rindfleisch, A. (2002). Materialism and well-being: a conflicting values perspective. J. Consum. Res. 29, 348–370. doi: 10.1086/344429

Chang, L., and Arkin, R. M. (2002). Materialism as an attempt to cope with uncertainty. Psychol. Mark. 19, 389–406. doi: 10.1002/mar.10016

Chaplin, L. N., and John, D. R. (2007). Growing up in a material world: age differences in materialism in children and adolescents. J. Consum. Res. 34, 480–493. doi: 10.1086/518546

Chaplin, L. N., and John, D. R. (2010). Interpersonal influences on adolescent materialism: a new look at the role of parents and peers. J. Consum. Psychol. 20, 176–184. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.02.002

Choi, J., and Geistfeld, L. V. (2004). A cross-cultural investigation of consumer e-shopping adoption. J. Econ. Psychol. 25, 821–838. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0212

Cohen, P., and Cohen, J. (1996). Life Values and Adolescent Mental Health. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dittmar, H., Bond, R., Hurst, M., and Kasser, T. (2014). The relationship between materialism and personal well-being: a meta-analysis. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 107, 879–924. doi: 10.1037/a0037409

Eastman, J. K., Goldsmith, R. E., and Flynn, L. R. (1999). Status consumption in consumer behavior: scale development and validation. J. Mark. Theory Pract. 7, 41–52. doi: 10.1080/10696679.1999.11501839

Fung, H. H., Isaacowitz, D. M., Lu, A. Y., and Li, T. (2010). Interdependent self-construal moderates the age-related negativity reduction effect in memory and visual attention. Psychol. Aging 25, 321–329. doi: 10.1037/a0019079

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., and Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychol. Sci. 10, 321–326. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00162

Goldsmith, R. E., and Clark, R. A. (2012). Materialism, status consumption, and consumer independence. J. Soc. Psychol. 152, 43–60. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2011.555434

Hanley, A., and Wilhelm, M. S. (1992). Compulsive buying: an exploration into self-esteem and money attitudes. J. Econ. Psychol. 13, 5–18. doi: 10.1016/0167-4870(92)90049-d

Harb, C., and Smith, P. B. (2008). Self-construals across cultures: beyond independence—interdependence. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 39, 178–197. doi: 10.1037/xge0000175

Hardin, E. E., Leong, F. T., and Bhagwat, A. A. (2004). Factor structure of the self-construal scale revisited: implications for the multidimensionality of self-construal. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 35, 327–345. doi: 10.1177/0022022104264125

Hui, C. H., and Triandis, H. C. (1986). Individualism-collectivism a study of cross-cultural researchers. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 17, 225–248. doi: 10.1177/0022002186017002006

Inglehart, R. F., Basanez, M., Basanez, M., and Moreno, A. (1998). Human Values and Beliefs: A Cross-Cultural Sourcebook. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

Isaksen, K. J., and Roper, S. (2012). The commodification of self-esteem: branding and British teenagers. Psychol. Mark. 29, 117–135. doi: 10.1002/mar.20509

Jensen, L., and Jensen, J. (1993). Family values, religiosity, and gender. Psychol. Rep. 73, 429–430. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1993.73.2.429

Jiang, J., Zhang, Y., Ke, Y., Hawk, S. T., and Qiu, H. (2015). Can’t buy me friendship? Peer rejection and adolescent materialism: implicit self-esteem as a mediator. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 58, 48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.01.001

Jiang, Y., Chen, Z., and Wyer, R. S. (2013). “Monetary reminders lead to exchange orientation and emotion suppression,” in NA - Advances in Consumer Research , Vol. 41, eds S. Botti and A. Labroo (Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research).

Kasser, T. (2003). The High Price of Materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Kasser, T. (2005). “Frugality, generosity, and materialism in children and adolescents,” in What Do Children Need to Flourish? The Search Institute Series on Developmentally Attentive Community and Society , eds K. A. Moore and L. H. Lippman (Boston, MA: Springer), 357–373. doi: 10.1007/0-387-23823-9_22

Kasser, T. (2016). Materialistic values and goals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 489–514. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033344

Kasser, T., and Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 65, 410–422. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.2.410

Kasser, T., and Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22, 280–287. doi: 10.1177/0146167296223006

La Barbera, P. A., and Gürhan, Z. (1997). The role of materialism, religiosity, and demographics in subjective well-being. Psychol. Mark. 14, 71–97. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6793(199701)14:1<71::aid-mar5>3.3.co;2-v

Leary, M. R., and Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: sociometer theory. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 32, 1–62. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(00)80003-9

Li, H. Z., Zhang, Z., Bhatt, G., and Yum, Y. O. (2006). Rethinking culture and self-construal: China as a middle land. J. Soc. Psychol. 146, 591–610. doi: 10.3200/socp.146.5.591-610

Lin, Z., Lin, Y., and Han, S. (2008). Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biol. Psychol. 77, 93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.002

Liviatan, I., Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2008). Interpersonal similarity as a social distance dimension: implications for perception of others’ actions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1256–1269. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.007

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.98.2.224

Mead, N. L., Caruso, E. M., Vohs, K., and Baumeister, R. F. (2013). “Monetary cues alter interpersonal harmony because they activate an exchange-orientation,” in Proceedings of the Association for the Consumer Research at Making a Difference with Metal Pieces: New Findings on Seeing, Possessing, & Losing Money , eds Y. Jiang and N. L. Mead (Chicago, IL).

Mick, D. G. (1996). Are studies of dark side variables confounded by socially desirable responding? The case of materialism. J. Consum. Res. 23, 106–119.

Opree, S. J., Buijzen, M., van Reijmersdal, E. A., and Valkenburg, P. M. (2011). Development and validation of the material values scale for children (MVS-c). Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 963–968. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.029

Richins, M. L., and Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values orientation for materialism and its measurement: scale development and validation. J. Consum. Res. 19, 303–316.

Rindfleisch, A., Burroughs, J. E., and Denton, F. (1997). Family structure, materialism, and compulsive consumption. J. Consum. Res. 23, 312–325.

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University.

Sangkhawasi, T., and Johri, L. M. (2007). Impact of status brand strategy on materialism in Thailand. J. Consum. Mark. 24, 275–282. doi: 10.1108/07363760710773094

Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1–65. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60281-6

Sheldon, K. M., and Kasser, T. (2008). Psychological threat and extrinsic goal striving. Motiv. Emot. 32, 37–45. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9081-5

Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 20, 580–591. doi: 10.1177/0146167294205014

Sivanathan, N., and Pettit, N. C. (2010). Protecting the self through consumption: status goods as affirmational commodities. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 564–570. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.01.006

Solberg, E. G., Diener, E., and Robinson, M. D. (2004). “Why are materialists less satisfied?” in Psychology and Consumer Culture: The Struggle for a Good Life in a Materialistic World , eds T. Kasser and A. D. Kanner (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 29–48. doi: 10.1037/10658-003

Sui, J., and Han, S. (2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychol. Sci. 18, 861–866. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01992.x

Sun, G., D’Alessandro, S., and Johnson, L. (2014). Traditional culture, political ideologies, materialism and luxury consumption in China. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 38, 578–585. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12117

Teng, F., Poon, K. T., Zhang, H., Chen, Z., Yang, Y., and Wang, X. (2016). Situational cuing of materialism triggers self-objectification among women (but not men): the moderating role of self-concept clarity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 97, 220–228. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.066

Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., and Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 60, 649–655. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.649

Van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., De Bouter, C., and Van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: behavioral consequences of self-construals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1093–1102. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1093

Wang, X., and Krumhuber, E. G. (2017). The love of money results in objectification. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 56, 354–372. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12158

Wicklund, R. A., and Gollwitzer, P. M. (1981). Symbolic self-completion, attempted influence, and self-deprecation. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2, 89–114. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp0202_2

Wong, N. Y. (1997). Suppose you own the world and no one knows? Conspicuous consumption, materialism and self. Adv. Consum. Res. 24, 197–203.

Zhou, F. (2009). “Income gap, materialism, and attitude toward the rich in developing countries,” in Peace Psychology in Asia , eds N. Noor and C. Montiel (New York, NY: Springer), 275–286. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-0143-9_15

Keywords : self-esteem, self-construal, materialistic values, collectivist culture, experiments

Citation: Zhang Y and Hawk ST (2019) Considering the Self in the Link Between Self-Esteem and Materialistic Values: The Moderating Role of Self-Construal. Front. Psychol. 10:1375. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01375

Received: 24 January 2019; Accepted: 27 May 2019; Published: 25 June 2019.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2019 Zhang and Hawk. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yan Zhang, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

materialism research

There’s no shame in being materialistic – it could benefit society

materialism research

Lecturer in Marketing, Lancaster University

Contributors

materialism research

Professor of International Management and Marketing, Vienna University of Economics and Business

materialism research

Senior Lecturer, University of Manchester

Disclosure statement

Charles Cui does not work for, consult, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organisation that would benefit from this article, and has disclosed no relevant affiliations beyond their academic appointment.

Bodo B. Schlegelmilch and Sandra Awanis do not work for, consult, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organisation that would benefit from this article, and have disclosed no relevant affiliations beyond their academic appointment.

Lancaster University provides funding as a founding partner of The Conversation UK.

View all partners

Materialism gets a bad press. There is an assumption that people who prioritise “things” are inherently selfish. The stereotype is that of highly materialistic people, living in a different world, where their priority is cash, possessions and status. But is the stereotype true? Our research reveals there are two sides to this story.

Highly materialistic people believe that owning and buying things are necessary means to achieve important life goals, such as happiness, success and desirability. However, in their quest to own more, they often sideline other important goals. Research shows that highly materialistic people tend to care less about the environment and other people than “non-materialists” do. These findings lead to the assumption that highly materialistic people are largely selfish and prefer to build meaningful relationships with “stuff”, as opposed to people.

But other research shows that materialism is a natural part of being human and that people develop materialistic tendencies as an adaptive response to cope with situations that make them feel anxious and insecure, such as a difficult family relationship or even our natural fear of death .

materialism research

Underlying desires

Materialism is not only found in particularly materialistic people. Even referring to people as “consumers” , as opposed to using other generic terms such as citizens, can temporarily activate a materialistic mindset. As materialism researchers James Burroughs and Aric Rindfleisch said:

Telling people to be less materialistic is like telling people that they shouldn’t enjoy sex or eat fatty foods. People can learn to control their impulses, but this does not remove the underlying desires.

As such, efforts directed towards eliminating materialism (taxing or banning advertising activities ) are unlikely to be effective. These anti-materialism views also limit business activities and places considerable tension between business and policy.

The caring materialists

Our research examined how materialism is perceived across cultures and it revealed that there is more to materialism than just self-gratification. In Asia, materialism is an important part of the “collectivistic” culture (where the emphasis is on relationships with others, in particular the groups a person belongs to).

Buying aspirational brands of goods and services is a common approach in the gift-giving traditions in East Asia. Across collectivistic communities, purchasing things that mirror the identity and style of people you regard as important can also help you to conform to social expectations that in turn blanket you with a sense of belonging. These behaviours are not unique to Asian societies. It’s just that the idea of materialism in the West is more often seen in sharp contrast to community values, rather than a part of it.

We also found that materialists in general are “meaning-seekers” rather than status seekers. They believe in the symbolic and signalling powers of products, brands and price tags. Materialists who also believe in community values use these cues to shed positive light onto themselves and others they care about, to meet social expectations, demonstrate belonging and even to fulfil their perceived social responsibilities. For example, people often flaunt their green and eco-friendly purchases of Tom’s shoes and Tesla cars in public to signal desirable qualities of altruism and social concern.

Reconciling material and collective interests

So how do we get an increasingly materialistic society to care more about the greater good (such as buying more ethically-sourced products or making more charity donations) and be less conspicuous and wasteful in its consumption? The answer is to look to our culture and what sort of collectivistic values it tries to teach us.

We found that a simple reminder of the community value that resonates with who we are as a society can help reduce materialistic tendencies. That said, the Asian and Western cultures tend to teach slightly different ideals of community value. Asian communities tend to pass on values that centre around interpersonal relationships (such as family duties). Western societies tend to pass on values that are abstract and spiritual (such as kindness, equality and social justice).

Unsurprisingly, many businesses have been quick to jump onto this bandwagon. Tear-jerking commercials from Thailand reminding people to buy insurance to protect loved ones, Christmas adverts reminding viewers to be kind to one another are just two examples. But nice commercials alone won’t be enough to do the job.

Social marketers and public policymakers should tap into society’s materialistic tendencies to promote well-meaning social programmes, such as refugee settlement, financial literacy programmes and food bank donations. The key is to promote these programmes in ways that materialists can engage with – through a public display of consumption that communicates social identity.

A perfect example is the Choose Love charity pop-up store in central London, where people get to purchase real products (blankets, children’s clothing, sleeping bags, sanitary pads) in a beautifully designed retail space akin to the Apple store, which are then distributed to refugees in Greece, Iraq and Syria.

Materialism undoubtedly has an ugly face but it is here to stay. Rather than focusing efforts to diminish it, individual consumers, businesses and policymakers should focus on using it for promoting collective interests that benefit wider society.

  • Consumerism
  • materialism
  • Black Friday

materialism research

Assistant Editor - 1 year cadetship

materialism research

Program Development Officer - Business Processes

materialism research

Executive Dean, Faculty of Health

materialism research

Lecturer/Senior Lecturer, Earth System Science (School of Science)

materialism research

Sydney Horizon Educators (Identified)

  • Newsletters

Site search

  • Israel-Hamas war
  • Home Planet
  • 2024 election
  • Supreme Court
  • TikTok’s fate
  • All explainers
  • Future Perfect

Filed under:

  • Neuroscience
  • Science of Everyday Life

A psychologist explains why materialism is making you unhappy

Share this story.

  • Share this on Facebook
  • Share this on Twitter
  • Share this on Reddit
  • Share All sharing options

Share All sharing options for: A psychologist explains why materialism is making you unhappy

I have all these bags with things in them, and yet I'm so unfulfilled.

Materialists lead unhappier lives — and are worse to the people around them. And it seems that social media might be fueling materialistic attitudes, too. This is all according to a fascinating interview the American Psychological Association posted in 2014 with Knox College psychologist Tim Kasser , whose research focuses on materialism and well-being.

Here are the best bits.

Materialists are sad, terrible people:

We know from research that materialism tends to be associated with treating others in more competitive, manipulative and selfish ways, as well as with being less empathetic ... [M]aterialism is associated with lower levels of well-being, less pro-social interpersonal behavior, more ecologically destructive behavior, and worse academic outcomes. It also is associated with more spending problems and debt ... We found that the more highly people endorsed materialistic values, the more they experienced unpleasant emotions, depression and anxiety, the more they reported physical health problems, such as stomachaches and headaches, and the less they experienced pleasant emotions and felt satisfied with their lives.

People become more materialistic when they feel insecure:

Research shows two sets of factors that lead people to have materialistic values. First, people are more materialistic when they are exposed to messages that suggest such pursuits are important ... Second, and somewhat less obvious — people are more materialistic when they feel insecure or threatened, whether because of rejection, economic fears or thoughts of their own death.

Materialism is linked to media exposure and national-advertising expenditures:

The research shows that the more that people watch television, the more materialistic their values are ... A study I recently published with psychologist Jean Twenge ... found that the extent to which a given year’s class of high school seniors cared about materialistic pursuits was predictable on the basis of how much of the U.S. economy came from advertising and marketing expenditures — the more that advertising dominated the economy, the more materialistic youth were.

Materialism is linked to social media use, too:

One study of American and Arab youth found that materialism is higher as social media use increases ... That makes sense, since most social media messages also contain advertising, which is how the social media companies make a profit.

Many psychologists think that materialists are unhappy because these people neglect their real psychological needs:

[M]aterialistic values are associated with living one’s life in ways that do a relatively poor job of satisfying psychological needs to feel free, competent and connected to other people. When people do not have their needs well-satisfied, they report lower levels of well-being and happiness, as well as more distress.

Check out the whole interview at the APA's website.

Will you support Vox today?

We believe that everyone deserves to understand the world that they live in. That kind of knowledge helps create better citizens, neighbors, friends, parents, and stewards of this planet. Producing deeply researched, explanatory journalism takes resources. You can support this mission by making a financial gift to Vox today. Will you join us?

We accept credit card, Apple Pay, and Google Pay. You can also contribute via

materialism research

Next Up In Life

Sign up for the newsletter today, explained.

Understand the world with a daily explainer plus the most compelling stories of the day.

Thanks for signing up!

Check your inbox for a welcome email.

Oops. Something went wrong. Please enter a valid email and try again.

materialism research

Where billions of cicadas will emerge this spring (and over the next decade), in one map

NTSB Investigator-in-Charge John Lovell examines the fuselage plug area of Alaska Airlines Flight 1282 Boeing 737-9 MAX.

Boeing’s problems were as bad as you thought

Two black-feathered chickens with red crests are in close-up, one in the foreground with its head turned and one eye looking at the viewer, the other slightly blurry in the background sitting down.

No one wants to think about pandemics. But bird flu doesn’t care.

The justices standing in their robes in the crowded House Chamber.

The Supreme Court: The most powerful, least busy people in Washington

The arrivals board at an airport showing delayed flights.

You could soon get cash for a delayed flight

materialism research

Baby Reindeer’s messy stalking has led to more messy stalking offscreen

School of Materialist Research 

New Materialist Methods and the Research Process

  • First Online: 18 December 2020

Cite this chapter

materialism research

  • Holly Thorpe 7 ,
  • Julie Brice 7 &
  • Marianne Clark 8  

Part of the book series: New Femininities in Digital, Physical and Sporting Cultures ((NFDPSC))

878 Accesses

1 Citations

In this chapter we discuss the challenges, opportunities, and considerations of putting new materialist theory into practice in empirical research. In this chapter we engage with literature from across a range of fields to provide an overview of the many ways that new materialisms are informing the research process and methodology, methods, and researcher positionality, reflexivity, and ethics. We begin by elaborating on how the onto-epistemology of new materialisms encourages alternative approaches to the research process. Following this, we map out some of the diverse ways that scholars are reengaging and reimagining research methods, including media analysis, interviews, participatory methods, autoethnography, arts-related research practices, embodied and movement-based methods, and transdisciplinarity and mixed methods. We conclude this chapter with a discussion of how new materialist theory encourages new questions and considerations of ethics, reflexivity, and the politics of knowledge production.

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=yM-Q4FmW6h8&list=PLdbxSLlj0ri04cOHxK37TfaQg0IAv6Znf&index=2

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Ackerly, B., & True, J. (2008). Reflexivity in practice: Power and ethics in feminist research on international relations. International Studies Review, 10 (4), 693–707.

Article   Google Scholar  

Ahmed, S. (2008). Open forum. Imaginary prohibitions. Some preliminary remarks on the founding gesture of the ‘new materialism. European Journal of Women’s Studies, 15 (1), 23–39.

Alaimo, S. (2008). Transcorporeal feminisms and the ethical space of nature. In S. Alaimo & S. Hekman (Eds.), Material feminisms (pp. 237–264). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

Google Scholar  

Alldred, P., & Fox, N. J. (2015). The sexuality- assemblages of young men: A new materialist analysis. Sexualities, 18 (8), 905–920.

Allen, L. (2018). Sexual choreographies of the classroom: Movement in sexuality education. Discourse: Studies in the Cultural Politics of Education, 39 (3), 347–360.

Allen-Collinson, J., & Leledaki, A. (2015). Sensing the outdoors: a visual and haptic phenomenology of outdoor exercise embodiment. Leisure Studies, 34(4), 457–470.

Åsberg, C. (2013). The timely ethics of posthuman gender studies. Feministische Studien, 31 (1), 7–12.

Back, L., & Puwar, N. (Eds.). (2012). Live methods . Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

Barad, K. (2003). Posthumanist performativity: Toward an understanding of how matter comes to matter. Signs, 28 (3), 801–831.

Barad, K. (2007). Meeting the universe halfway: Quantum physics and the entanglement of matter and meaning . Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Barad, K. (2010). Quantum entanglements and hauntological relations of inheritance: Dis/continuities, spacetime infoldings, and justice-to-come. Derrida Today, 3 (2), 240–268.

Barad, K. (2014). Diffracting diffraction: Cutting together-apart. Parallax, 20 (3), 168–187.

Barrett, E., & Bolt, B. (Eds.). (2013). Carnal knowledge: Towards a ‘new materialism’ through the arts . New York, NY: I.B. Tauris.

Bastian, M., Jones, O., Moore, N., & Roe, E. (Eds.). (2017). Participatory research in more-than-human worlds . New York, NY: Routledge.

Beausoleil, E. (2015). Embodying an ethics of response-ability. Borderlands, 14 (2), 1–16.

Bozalek, V., & Zembylas, M. (2017). Diffraction or reflection? Sketching the contours of two methodologies in educational research. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 30 (2), 111–127.

Braidotti, R. (2013). The posthuman . Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.

Braidotti, R. (2019a). A theoretical framework for the critical posthumanities. Theory, Culture & Society, 36 (6), 31–61.

Brice, J. E., Clark, M., & Thorpe, H. (2020). Feminist collaborative becomings: An entangled process of knowing through fitness objects. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health. https://doi.org/10.1080/2159676X.2020.1820560

Buller, H. (2015). Animal geographies II: Methods. Progress in Human Geography, 39 (3), 374–384.

Büscher, M., Urry, J., & Witchger, K. (Eds.). (2010). Mobile methods . Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge.

Cardinal, A. (2019). Participatory video: An apparatus for ethnically researching literacy, power and embodiment. Computers and Composition, 53 , 34–46.

Clark, M. (2020). Re-imagining the dancing body with and through Barad. In J. Newman, H. Thorpe, & D. Andrews (Eds.), Sport, physical culture and the moving body: Materialisms, technologies, and ecologies (pp. 209–228). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Clark, M., & Thorpe, H. (2020). Towards diffractive ways of knowing women’s moving bodies: A Baradian experiment with the fitbit/motherhood entanglement. Sociology of Sport, 37 (1), 12–26.

Coleman, R., & Osgood, J. (2019). PhEMaterialist encounters with glitter: The materialisation of ethics, politics and care in arts-based research. Reconceptualizing Educational Research Methodology, 10 (2–3), 61–86.

Coleman, R., Page, T., & Palmer, H. (2019). Feminist new materialist practice: The mattering of methods. Mai: Feminism and Visual Culture . https://maifeminism.com/feminist-new-materialisms-the-mattering-of-methods-editors-note/

Coleman, R., & Ringrose, J. (2013). Introduction: Deleuze and research methodologies. In R. Coleman & J. Ringrose (Eds.), Deleuze and research methodologies (pp. 1–22). Edinburgh, Scotland: Edinburgh University Press.

Connolly, W. E. (2013). The ‘new materialism’ and the fragility of things. Millennium, 41 (3), 399–412.

Coole, D., & Frost, S. (2010b). Introducing the new materialisms. In D. Coole & S. Frost (Eds.), New materialisms: Ontology, agency and politics (pp. 1–42). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Daley, A. (2010). Reflections on reflexivity and critical reflection as critical research practices. Affilia, 25 (1), 68–82.

Davies, B. (2014). Reading anger in early childhood intra-actions: A diffractive analysis. Qualitative Inquiry, 20 (6), 734–741.

de Freitas, E. (2016). Calculating matter and recombinant subjects: The infinitesimal and the fractal fold. Cultural Studies↔Critical Methodologies, 16 , 462–470.

de Freitas, E. (2017). Karen Barad’s quantum ontology and posthuman ethics: Rethinking the concept of relationality. Qualitative Inquiry, 23 (9), 741–748.

Deleuze, G., & Guattari, F. (1987). A thousand plateaus: Capitalism and schizophrenia . Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Dickinson, S. (2017). Writing sensation: Critical autoethnography in posthumanism. In J. Holman & M. Pruyn (Eds.), Creative selves/Creative cultures (pp. 77–92). Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan.

Dolphijn, R., & van der Tuin, I. (2012). New materialism: Interviews and cartographies . Ann Arbor, MI: Open Humanities Press.

Book   Google Scholar  

Ellingson, L. (2017). Embodiment in qualitative research . New York, NY: Routledge.

Flynn, N. (2016). Performativity and metaphor in new materialist media theory. Networking Knowledge: Journal of the MeCCSA Postgraduate Network, 9 (1).

Fox, N., & Alldred, P. (2015). New materialist social inquiry: Designs, methods and the research-assemblage. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 18 (4), 399–414.

Fox, N., & Alldred, P. (2016a). Sociology and the new materialism: Theory, research, action . London, UK: SAGE.

Fox, N., & Alldred, P. (2018b). Mixed methods, materialism and the micropolitics of the research-assemblage. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 21 (2), 194–202.

Francombe-Webb, J. (2017). Methods that move: Exer-gaming and embodied experiences of femininity. In M. Giardina & M. Donnelly (Eds.), Physical culture, ethnography and the body (pp. 183–196). London, UK: Routledge.

Fullagar, S. (2017). Post-qualitative inquiry and the new materialist turn: Implications for sport, health and physical culture research. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 9 (2), 247–257.

Fullagar, S. (2020). Diffracting mind-body relations: Feminist materialism and the entanglement of physical culture in women’s recovery from depression. In J. Newman, H. Thorpe, & D. Andrews (Eds.), Sport, physical culture and the moving body: Materialisms, technologies, and ecologies (pp. 170–192). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Fullagar, S. (2021). Re-turning to embodied matters and movement through postqualitative inquiries. In K. Murris (Ed.), Navigating the postqualitative, new materialist and critical posthumanist terrain across disciplines: An introductory guide. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

Fullagar, S., Pavlidis, A., & Stadler, R. (2017). Collaborative writing as rhizomatic practice: Critical moments of (un)doing doctoral supervision. Knowledge Cultures, 5 (4), 23–41.

Gauntlett, D. (2011). Making is connecting: The social meaning of creativity, from DIY and knitting to YouTube and Web 2.0 . Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.giardina.

Geerts, E., & van der Tuin, I. (2016). The feminist future or reading diffractively: How Barad’s methodology replaces conflict-based readings of Beauvoir and Irigaray. Rhizomes: Cultural Studies in Emerging Knowledge , (30).

Giardina, M. D. (2017). (Post?)qualitative inquiry in sport, exercise, and health: Notes on a methodologically contested present. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 9 (2), 258–270.

Giardina, M. D., & Newman, J. (2011). What is the ‘physical’ in physical cultural studies? Sociology of Sport Journal, 28 (1), 36–63.

Gildersleeve, R. (2017). The neoliberal academy of the anthropocene and the retaliation of the lazy academic. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies, 17 (3), 286–293.

Guttorm, H., Hohti, R., & Paakaari, A. (2015). “Do the next thing”: An interview with Elizabeth Adams St. Pierre on post-qualitative methodology. Reconceptualizing Education Research Methodology, 6 (1).

Hammoor, C. (2019). Playful pedagogy: Autoethnography in the anthropocene. In C. Taylor & A. Bayley (Eds.), Posthumanism and higher education (pp. 281–291). Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Handforth, R., & Taylor, C. A. (2016). Doing academic writing differently: A feminist bricolage. Gender and Education, 28 , 627–643.

Haraway, D. (1988). Situated knowledges: The science question in feminism and the privilege of partial perspectives. Feminist Studies, 14 (3), 575–599.

Haraway, D. (1992). Otherworldly conversations; Terrain topics; local terms. Science as Culture, 3 (1), 64–98.

Haraway, D. (1997). Modest_Witness@Second_Millennium.FemaleMan_Meets_OncoMouse: Feminism and Technoscience . New York, NY: Routledge.

Harding, S. (1986). The science question in feminism . Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Harding, S. (Ed.). (1987). Feminism and methodology: Social science issues . Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

Harding, S. (1996). Standpoint epistemology (a feminist version): How social disadvantage creates epistemic advantage. In S. Turner (Ed.), Social theory and sociology: The classics and beyond (pp. 146–160). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Harding, S. (2006). Science and social inequality: Feminist and postcolonial issues . Chicago, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Heddon, D. (2017). Con-versing: Listening, speaking, turning. In M. Bastian, O. Jones, N. Moore, & E. Roe (Eds.), Participatory research in more-than-human worlds (pp. 192–208). New York, NY: Routledge.

Hemmings, C. (2011). Why stories matter: The political grammar of feminist theory . Durham, NC/London, UK: Duke University Press.

Hickey-Moody, A., & Page, T. (2015). Arts, pedagogy and cultural resistance: New materialisms . London, UK: Rowman & Littlefield International.

Hickey-Moody, A., Palmer, H., & Sayers, E. (2016). Diffractive pedagogies: Dancing across new materialist imaginaries. Gender and Education, 28 (2), 213–229.

Hodgetts, T., & Lorimer, J. (2015). Methodologies for animals’ geographies: Cultures, communication and genomics. Cultural Geographies, 22 (2), 285–295.

Hughes, C., & Lury, C. (2013). Re-turning feminist methodologies: From a social to an ecological epistemology. Gender and Education, 25 (6), 786–799.

Hultman, K., & Lenz-Taguchi, H. (2010). Challenging anthropocentric analysis of visual data: A relational materialist methodological approach to educational research. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 23 (5), 525–542.

Humberstone, B. (2011). Embodiment and social action in nature-based sport: Spiritual spaces. Leisure Studies, 30 (4), 495–512.

Irni, S. (2010). Ageing apparatuses at work: Transdisciplinary negotiations of sex, age and materiality . Åbo, Finland: Åbo Akademi University Press.

Irni, S. (2013a). Sex, power and ontology: Exploring the performativity of hormones. Nordic Journal of Feminist and Gender Research, 21 (1), 41–56.

Irni, S. (2013b). The politics of materiality: Affective encounters in a transdisciplinary debate. European Journal of Women’s Studies, 20 (4), 347–360.

Jackson, A., & Mazzei, L. (2013). Plugging one text into another: Thinking with theory in qualitative research. Qualitative Inquiry, 19 (4), 261–271.

Jeffrey, A. (2020). Women’s contemporary yoga lifestyles: An embodied ethnography of becoming. PhD thesis. University of Waikato. Available from: https://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/handle/10289/13578

Jeffrey, A., Barbour, K., & Thorpe, H. (Under Review). Entangled yoga bodies.

Jette, S., Esmonde, K., Andrews, D., & Pluim, C. (2020). Big bodies, big data: Unpacking the FitnessGram Black Box. In J. Newman, H. Thorpe, & D. Andrews (Eds.), Sport, physical culture and the moving body: Materialisms, technologies, and ecologies (pp. 131–150). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

King, S. (2020). Towards a multispecies sport studies. In J. Newman, H. Thorpe, & D. Andrews (Eds.), Sport, physical culture and the moving body: Materialisms, technologies, and ecologies (pp. 193–208). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Koro-Ljungberg, M. (2015). Reconceptualizing qualitative research: Methodologies without methodology . Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.

Koro-Ljungberg, M., Löytönen, T., & Tesar, M. (2017). Disrupting data in qualitative inquiry: Entanglements with the post-critical and post-anthropocentric (post-anthropocentric inquiry) . New York, NY: Peter Lang Publishing Inc.

Koro-Ljungberg, M., & MacLure, M. (2013). Provocations, re-un-visions, death, and other possibilities of ‘data. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies, 13 , 219–222.

Koro-Ljungberg, M., Yendol-Hoppey, D., Smith, J., & Hayes, S. (2009). (E)pistemological awareness, instantiation of methods, and uninformed methodological ambiguity in qualitative research projects. Educational Researcher, 38 , 687–699.

Lather, P. (2007). Getting lost: Feminist practices toward a double(d) science . Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

Lather, P. (2016a). Top ten+ list: (Re)thinking ontology in (post)qualitative research. Cultural Studies ⇔ Critical Methodologies, 16 (2), 125–131.

Lather, P. (2016b). Post-face: Cultural studies of numeracy. Cultural Studies ⇔ Critical Methodologies, 16 (5), 502–505.

Lather, P., & St. Pierre, E. (2013). Post-qualitative research. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 26 (6), 629–633.

Law, J. (2004). After method: The mess of social science research . London, UK: Routledge.

Lenz-Taguchi, H. (2012). A diffractive and Deleuzian approach to analysing interview data. Feminist Theory, 13 (3), 265–281.

Lenz-Taguchi, H. (2013). ‘Becoming molecular girl’: Transforming subjectivities in collaborative doctoral research studies as micro-politics in the academy. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 26 (9), 1101–1116.

Levy, G., Halse, C., & Wright, J. (2016). Down the methodological rabbit hole: Thinking diffractively with resistant data. Qualitative Research, 16 (2), 183–197.

Liao, J., & Markula, P. (2016). “The only thing i am guilty of is taking too many jump shots”: A Deleuzian media analysis of Diana Taurasi’s drug charge in 2010. Sociology of Sport Journal, 33 (2), 169–179.

Lupton, D. (2019b). ‘Things that matter’: Poetic inquiry and more-than-human health literacy. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health , 1–16

Lupton, D. (2019d). Australian women’s use of health and fitness apps and wearable devices: a feminist new materialism analysis. Feminist Media Studies , 1–16

Lupton, D. (2020). The story completion method and more-than-human theory: Finding and using health information. SAGE Research Methods Cases.

Lury, C., & Wakeford, N. (Eds.). (2013). Inventive methods: The happening of the social . London, UK: Routledge.

Lynch, M. (2000). Against reflexivity as an academic virtue and source of privileged knowledge. Theory, Culture and Society, 17 (3), 26–54.

MacLure, M. (2013a). Researching without representation? Language and materiality in post-qualitative methodology. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 26 (6), 658–667.

Mancini, C., van der Linden, J., Bryan, J., & Stuart, A. (2012). Exploring interspecies sensemaking: Dog tracking semiotics and multispecies ethnography. In Proceedings of the 2012 ACM Conference on Ubiquitous Computing, Ubicomp 2012 , 5–8 September 2012, Pittsburgh, PA, pp. 143–152.

Manning, E., & Massumi, B. (2014). Thought in the act: Passages in the ecology of experience . Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Markula, P. (2011). “Folding”: A feminist intervention in mindful fitness. In E. Kennedy & P. Markula (Eds.), Women and exercise: The body, health and consumerism (pp. 60–78). London, UK/New York, NY: Routledge.

Markula, P. (2014). The moving body and social change. Cultural Studies? Critical Methodologies, 14 (5), 483–495.

Markula, P. (2019). What is new about new materialism for sport sociology? Reflections on body, movement, and culture. Sociology of Sport Sociology, 36 , 1–11.

Markula, P. (2020). Contextualizing the material, moving body. In J. Newman, H. Thorpe, & D. Andrews (Eds.), Sport, physical culture and the moving body: Materialisms, technologies and ecologies (pp. 47–68). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Marn, T. M., & Wolgemuth, J. R. (2016). Purposeful entanglements: A new materialist analysis of transformative interviews. Qualitative Inquiry, 23 (5), 365–374.

Martin, A., & Kamberelis, G. (2013). Mapping not tracing: Qualitative educational research with political teeth. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 26 (6), 668–679.

Masny, D. (2013). Rhizomatic pathways in qualitative research. Qualitative Inquiry, 19 , 339–348.

Mayes, E. (2019). The mis/use of ‘voice’ in (post)qualitative research with children and young people: Histories, politics and ethics. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 32 (1), 1191–1209.

Mazzei, L. (2013). A voice without organs: Interviewing in posthumanist research. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 26 (6), 732–740.

McDougall, A., Goldszmidt, M., Kinsella, E., Smith, S., & Lingard, L. (2016). Collaboration and entanglement: An actor-network theory analysis of team-based intraprofessional care for patients with advanced health failure. Social Science and Medicine, 164 , 108–117.

McKnight, L. (2016). Swimming lessons: Learning, new materialisms, posthumanism, and post qualitative research emerge through a pool poem. Journal of Curriculum and Pedagogy, 13 (3), 195–205.

Mol, A. (2002). The body multiple: Ontology in medical practice . Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Monforte, J. (2018). What is new in new materialism for a newcomer? Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 10 (3), 378–190.

Monforte, J., & Smith, B. (2020). Conventional and postqualitative research: An invitation to dialogue. Qualitative Inquiry. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077800420962469

Murris, K. (Ed.). (2021). Navigating the postqualitative, new materialist and critical posthumanist terrain across disciplines: An introductory guide. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

Neimanis, A. (2018). Nature represents itself: Bibliophilia in a changing climate. In V. Kirby (Ed.), What if culture was nature all along? (pp. 179–198). Edinburgh, Scotland: Edinburgh University Press.

Nordstrom, S. N. (2018). Antimethodology: Postqualitative genertive conventions. Qualitative Inquiry, 24 (3), 215–226.

Olive, R. (2020). Thinking the social through myself: Reflexivity in research practice. In B. Humberstone & H. Prince (Eds.), Research methods in outdoor studies . New York, NY: Routledge.

Olive, R., & Thorpe, H. (2017). Feminist ethnography and physical culture: Towards reflexive, political, and collaborative methods. In Physical culture, ethnography and the body . New York, NY: Routledge.

Palmer, F. (2016). Stories of Haka and women’s rugby in Aotearoa New Zealand: Weaving identities and ideologies together. The International Journal of the History of Sport, 33 (17), 2169–2184.

Parikka, J. (2013). Afterword: Cultural techniques and media studies. Theory, Culture and Society, 30 (6), 147–159.

Parry, D., & Johnson, C. (2007). Contextualizing leisure research to encompass complexity in lived leisure experience: The need for creative analytic practice. Leisure Sciences, 29 (2), 119–130.

Patai, D. (1994). Response: When method becomes power. In A. Gitlen (Ed.), Power and method (pp. 61–73). New York, NY: Routledge.

Pels, D. (2000). Reflexivity: One step up. Theory, Culture and Society, 17 (3), 1–25.

Phillips, D. K., & Larson, M. L. (2013). The teacher-student writing conference reimaged: Entangled becoming-writing conferencing. Gender and Education, 25 (6), 722–737.

Pigott, J., & Lyons, A. (2017). Shadows, undercurrents and the Aliveness Machines . In M. Bastian, O. Jones, N. Moore, & E. Roe (Eds.), Participatory research in more-than-human worlds (pp. 241–160). New York, NY: Routledge.

Pillow, W. (2003). Confession, catharsis, or cure? Rethinking the uses of reflexivity as methodological power in qualitative research. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education , 16(2), 175–196

Pringle, R. (2020). What can new materialisms do for the critical study of sport and physical culture (Who does this book think it is?). In J. Newman, H. Thorpe, & D. Andrews (Eds.), Sport, physical culture and the moving body: Materialisms, technologies, and ecologies (pp. 321–334). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Pringle, R., & Thorpe, H. (2017). Theory and Reflexivity. In M. Silk, D. L. Andrews, & H. Thorpe (Eds.), Routledge Handbook of Physical Cultural Studies (pp. 68–78). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

Pryke, M., Rose, G., & Whatmore, S. (2003). Using social theory: Thinking through research . London, UK: SAGE.

Rautio, P. (2013). Children who carry stones in their pockets: On autotelic material practices in everyday life. Children’s Geographies, 11 (4), 394–408.

Ray, J. (2019). The postqualitative turn in physical cultural studies. Leisure Sciences, 41 (1), 91–107.

Reade, J. (2020). Keeping it raw on the ‘gram: Authenticity, relatability and digital intimacy in fitness cultures on Instagram. New Media & Society .

Rekret, P. (2016). A critique of new materialism: Ethics and ontology. Subjectivity, 9 , 225–245.

Rich, E., Lewis, S., & Miah, A. (2020). Digital health technologies, body pedagogies and material-discursive relations of young people’s learning about health. In D. Leahy, K. Fitzpatrick, & J. Wright (Eds.), Social theory and health education: Forging new insights in research . New York. NY: Routledge.

Safron, C. (2019). Reimagining health and fitness materials: An affective inquiry into collaging. Reconceptualizing Education Research Methodology, 2–3 (2), 40–58.

Scott, G., & Garner, R. (2013). Doing qualitative research: Design, methods and technologies . Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

Sehgal, M. (2014). Diffractive propositions: Reading Alfred North Whitehead with Donna Haraway and Karen Barad. Parallax, 20 (3), 188–201.

Shelton, S. A., Guyotte, K. W., & Flint, M. A. (2019). (Wo)monstrous suturing: Woman doctoral students cutting together/apart. Reconceptualizing Education Research Methodology, 2–3 (2), 111–146.

Shomura, C. (2017). Exploring the promise of new materialisms. Laterality, 6 (1), 1–7.

Søndergaard, D. M. (2016). New materialist analyses of virtual gaming, distributed violence, and recreational aggression. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies , 16(2), 162–172.

Springgay, S., & Truman, S. (2017). Walking methodologies in a more-than-human world: WalkingLab . New York, NY: Routledge.

Springgay, S., & Truman, S. (2018). On the need for methods beyond proceduralism: Speculative middles, (in)tensions and response-ability in research. Qualitative Inquiry, 24 (3), 203–214.

St. Pierre, E. (2011). Post qualitative research: The critique and the coming after. In N. Denzin & L. Yvonna (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of qualitative research (pp. 611–635). Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.

St. Pierre, E. (2015). Practice for the ‘new’ in the new empiricisms, the new materialisms and post qualitative inquiry. In N. K. Denzin & M. D. Giardina (Eds.), Qualitative inquiry and the politics of research (pp. 75–96). London, UK: Routledge.

St. Pierre, E. (2016). The empirical and the new empiricisms. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies, 16 (2), 111–124.

St. Pierre, E. (2019). Post Qualitative Inquiry, the refusal of method, and the risk of the new . Paper presented at the International Congress of Qualitative Inquiry, Urbana-Champaign, IL.

St. Pierre, E., Jackson, A., & Mazzei, L. (2016). New empiricisms and new materialisms: Conditions for new inquiry. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies, 16 (2), 99–110.

Strom, K., Ringrose, J., Osgood, J., & Renold, E. (2019). PhEmaterialism: Response-able Research & Pedagogy. Reconceptualizing Educational Research Methodology, 3 (2), 1–39.

Thomas, H., & Ahmed, J. (Eds.). (2008). Cultural bodies: Ethnography and theory . Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.

Thorpe, H. (2014). Moving bodies beyond the social/biological divide: Toward theoretical and transdisciplinary adventures. Sport, Education and Society , 19(5), 666–686.

Thorpe, H. (2016). Athletic women’s experiences of amenorrhea: Biomedical technologies, somatic ethics and embodied subjectivities. Sociology of Sport Journal , 33(1), 1–13.

Thorpe, H., & Clark, M. (2019). Gut Feminism, new materialisms and sportwomen’s embodied health: The case of RED-S in endurance athletes. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health , 12(1), 1–17.

Thorpe, H., Clark, M., Brice, J., & Sims, S. (2020). The transdisciplinary health research apparatus: A Baradian account of knowledge boundaries and beyond. Health , 1–24. 

Toffoletti, K., & Thorpe, H. (2020). Bodies, gender, and digital affect in fitspiration media. Feminist Media Studies , 1–18.

Tompkins, K. W. (2016). On the limits and promise of new materialist philosophy. Laterality, 5 (1).

Ulmer, J. (2017). Posthumanism as research methodology: Inquiry in the Anthropocene. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 30 (9), 832–848.

van der Tuin, I. (2014). Diffraction as a methodology for feminist onto-epistemology: On encountering Chantal Chawaf and posthuman interpellation. Parallax, 20 , 231–244.

van Ingen, C. (2016). Getting lost as a way of knowing: The art of boxing within Shape Your Life. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 8 (5), 472–486.

Warfield, K. (2016). Making the cut: Realist examinations of selfies and touch. Social Media and Society, 2 (2), 1–10.

Wolgemuth, J. R. (2016). Driving the paradigm: (Failing to Teach) methodological ambiguity, fluidity, and resistance in qualitative research. Qualitative Inquiry, 22 (6), 518–525.

Woodward, S. (2015). Object interviews, material imaginings and ‘unsettling’ methods: Interdisciplinary approaches to understanding materials and material culture. Qualitative Research, 16 (4), 359–374.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

University of Waikato, Hamilton, New Zealand

Holly Thorpe & Julie Brice

University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia

Marianne Clark

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Holly Thorpe .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Thorpe, H., Brice, J., Clark, M. (2020). New Materialist Methods and the Research Process. In: Feminist New Materialisms, Sport and Fitness. New Femininities in Digital, Physical and Sporting Cultures. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-56581-7_2

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-56581-7_2

Published : 18 December 2020

Publisher Name : Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-56580-0

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-56581-7

eBook Packages : Social Sciences Social Sciences (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

ScienceDaily

Social-media break has huge impact on young women's body image, study finds

Psychology prof says online exposure to idealized images creates 'infinite' opportunities for comparison.

There's a large and growing body of evidence pointing to potentially negative impacts of social media on mental health, from its addictive nature to disruptions in sleep patterns to effects on body image. Now, a new study coming out of York University's Faculty of Health found young women who took a social media break for as little as one week had a significant boost in self-esteem and body image -- particularly those most vulnerable to thin-ideal internalization.

"The statistician inside me was excited -- we don't often see effect sizes this large in my area of psychology research because human behaviour is complicated and there's lots of variability," says Psychology Professor Jennifer Mills, co-author of the paper. "We hope this study can be used to help protect young people and influence social media companies to give users more agency in how they interact with these platforms."

The paper, out this week in the journal Body Image , is thought to be the first to look specifically at social-media breaks and body image. Mills, whose lab has been on the forefront of this kind of research, collaborated on the paper with graduate researcher Lindsay Samson and undergraduate Olivia Smith, both students at York. They expected that there might be recruitment challenges, but it turns out there was enthusiasm for taking a social media pause among the 66 first-year female undergraduates who participated. Half were instructed to continue their social media as per usual, while the other half were given instructions to refrain from Instagram, Facebook, Twitter, and TikTok and other social media use for one week. They conducted baseline surveys before the experiment, and tested the participants again after the week was over.

"There's natural variability in how people feel about their bodies and about themselves in general, so we took that into account statistically, and even after that there were still significant differences between the groups after one week," says Mills, who is also the director of clinical training for the graduate psychology program.

The differences in the social media landscape are remarkable compared to when Mills started researching eating disorders and the effects of media, like magazines aimed at women.

"Back then, you could only spend so many minutes or hours looking at fashion and beauty magazines and they only came out once a month. There was a finite amount of content that you would be exposed to. With social media it's infinite. It's always new and novel, which triggers our brain's reward system that makes us want more and more of something."

Mills says the improvements found in this study might be explained both by women spending far less time engaging in behaviours known to have a detrimental effect, such as comparisons with others, but they may have also replaced social media with healthier behaviours.

"If we're spending more time in real life, socializing with friends, getting sleep, getting outdoors, getting exercise, there could be secondary behaviours that fill the void left by social media. Future research will try to disentangle that."

  • Social Psychology
  • Relationships
  • Learning Disorders
  • Popular Culture
  • Media and Entertainment
  • Surveillance
  • Social Issues
  • Philosophy of mind
  • Public health
  • Circadian rhythm sleep disorder
  • Sociobiology
  • Environmental impact assessment
  • Social psychology

Story Source:

Materials provided by York University . Note: Content may be edited for style and length.

Journal Reference :

  • Olivia E. Smith, Jennifer S. Mills, Lindsay Samson. Out of the loop: Taking a one-week break from social media leads to better self-esteem and body image among young women . Body Image , 2024; 49: 101715 DOI: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2024.101715

Cite This Page :

Explore More

  • Countries' Plans to Remove CO2 Not Enough
  • Toward Robots With Human-Level Touch Sensitivity
  • 'Doubling' in Origin of Cancer Cells
  • New Catalyst for Using Captured Carbon
  • Random Robots Are More Reliable
  • Significant Discovery in Teleportation Research
  • Orangutan Treats Wound With Pain-Relieving Plant
  • 75,000-Year-Old Neanderthal from Burial Cave
  • Anticoagulant With an On-Off Switch
  • Sleep Resets Brain Connections -- At First

Trending Topics

Strange & offbeat.

COMMENTS

  1. Materialism

    materialism, in philosophy, the view that all facts (including facts about the human mind and will and the course of human history) are causally dependent upon physical processes, or even reducible to them.. The word materialism has been used in modern times to refer to a family of metaphysical theories (i.e., theories of the nature of reality) that can best be defined by saying that a theory ...

  2. Materialism and Well-Being Revisited: The Impact of Personality

    The negative association between materialism and well-being is currently almost an axiom in psychology and consumer research. In their extensive meta-analysis Dittmar et al. showed that the results of empirical studies concerning this association are consistent and indicate modest negative correlations between various measures of materialism and various aspects of well-being (the average ...

  3. Materialistic cues make us miserable: A meta‐analysis of the

    The present research is the first systematic review of experimental research that has looked at the causal effects of materialism on individual and societal well-being. The results from the analyses provide empirical evidence indicating that materialistic cues cause lower individual and societal well-being.

  4. Frontiers

    Therefore, the question of what causes materialism has attracted much research attention. It has been consistently found that self-esteem is a key variable that negatively predicts materialistic values (e.g., Braun and Wicklund, 1989; Hanley and Wilhelm, 1992; Chang and Arkin, 2002; Solberg et al., 2004; Chaplin and John, 2007, 2010). However ...

  5. Psychological causes, correlates, and consequences of materialism

    Materialism has a long history in consumer research, and the volume of research continues to expand rapidly. In this article, we review extant research on materialism, with a particular focus on research in the last 10 years. We structure the review around the antecedents and consequences of materialism.

  6. Research Review Materialism pathways: The processes that create and

    Research into the role of daily events in materialism processes is also needed, in the context of both the developmental and the reinforcement models. There is a strong tradition of experimental studies that manipulate simulated daily events or experiences among young adults (e.g., social exclusion, powerlessness).

  7. New materialist social inquiry: designs, methods and the research

    View PDF. This paper discusses issues of research design and methods in new materialist social inquiry, an approach that is attracting increasing interest across the social sciences as an alternative to either realist or constructionist ontologies. New materialism de-privileges human agency, focusing instead upon how assemblages of the animate ...

  8. Thinking With New Materialism in Qualitative Case Studies

    Informed by post-structuralist, feminist, post-colonialist and queer theories they reject the earlier historical materialism of Hegel and Marx, ... Sociology and the new materialism, theory, research, action. Sage. Crossref. Google Scholar. Fox N. J., Alldred P. (2018). Mixed methods, materialism and the micropolitics of the research assemblage.

  9. Materialism and Quality of Life

    To date, extant research on materialism has traditionally focused on individuals' pursuit of material objects. Recently, however, materialism scholars have begun to examine the impact of discretionary experiential purchases upon quality of life (Carter & Gilovich, 2010, 2013; Schmitt et al., 2015).

  10. Materialism

    Although materialism research has concentrated on the individual-level consequences (e.g., lowered life satisfaction, heightened anxiety) of valuing the acquisition of material objects, some scholars note that materialism also has important societal ramifications.

  11. Experientialism, Materialism, and the Pursuit of Happiness

    Abstract. Previous research indicates that materialistic aspirations are negatively associated with happiness and psychological health. Recent research extends these findings by demonstrating that allocating discretionary resources toward life experiences makes people happier than allocating discretionary resources toward material possessions.

  12. (PDF) What is Materialism? History and Concepts

    positively, materialism names the branch of philosophical worldviews that identify. being (the " ὄντος" of ontology) with matter, understood in its broadest sense as. changeability and ...

  13. Doing new materialist data analysis: a Spinozo-Deleuzian ethological

    Introduction: new materialism and social research. Across the social sciences, new materialist perspectives are informing research (Coole & Frost, Citation 2010; Fox & Alldred, Citation 2017).This 'turn to matter' in empirical research has been hampered, however, by a lack of exploration of how ontology translates into methodology and thence to specific research methods.

  14. COVID may have made us less materialistic

    Research has found that materialism leads to lower levels of happiness and life satisfaction, as well as causing negative moods and anxiety. Yet popular culture and social media make materialism ...

  15. What Psychology Says About Materialism and the Holidays

    Kasser: The connection between materialism and well-being is the longest-standing strand of research in the materialism literature. My colleagues at the University of Sussex and I recently published a meta-analysis that showed the negative relationship between materialism and well-being was consistent across all kinds of measures of materialism ...

  16. The association between materialism and perceived ...

    Future research into materialism should, therefore, adopt a longitudinal design that will elucidate the casual mechanisms that contribute to the development of materialism. Building on the findings of the current study, we suggest that the role of self-esteem, anxious attachment and family relationships in this process represent promising ...

  17. (PDF) The Relationship Between Materialism and Personal ...

    This meta-analysis investigates the relationship between individuals' materialistic orientation and their. personal well-being. Theoretical approaches in psychology agree that prioritizing money ...

  18. Matter: Journal of New Materialist Research

    Matter: Journal of New Materialist Research is an interdisciplinary forum for publishing original research that brings the multiple to the individual, social, and symbolic levels, and that resignifies the relation between matter and discourse. Thinking through and beyond the concept of difference as hierarchical and exclusionary, it transforms ...

  19. There's no shame in being materialistic

    Our research examined how materialism is perceived across cultures and it revealed that there is more to materialism than just self-gratification. In Asia, materialism is an important part of the ...

  20. A psychologist explains why materialism is making you unhappy

    Materialism is linked to media exposure and national-advertising expenditures: The research shows that the more that people watch television, the more materialistic their values are ...

  21. The School of Materialist Research

    Special Programs. Running on Cargo. The School of Materialist Research.

  22. Materialism is detrimental to academic engagement: Evidence ...

    Extensive research has shown that materialism is detrimental for one's well-being. However, little is known about how materialism is associated with learning-related outcomes. In Study 1 (n = 466), we conducted a survey study and found that students who scored high in materialism had lower levels of academic engagement. In Study 2 (n = 59), we used the Linguistic Inquiry Word Count (LIWC ...

  23. The Art of Knowing Things

    A physicist by training, Hideo explores a nexus of conventional scientific research, craft (as a dedicated ceramist and occasional textile maker), interdisciplinary humanities (from a home base of aesthetic philosophy, new materialism, and literary theory), and integrative education. Hideo teaches and directs a research laboratory at Stanford ...

  24. New Materialist Methods and the Research Process

    Abstract. In this chapter we discuss the challenges, opportunities, and considerations of putting new materialist theory into practice in empirical research. In this chapter we engage with literature from across a range of fields to provide an overview of the many ways that new materialisms are informing the research process and methodology ...

  25. Social-media break has huge impact on young women's body ...

    Now, a new study has found that young women who took a social media break for as little as one week had a significant boost in self-esteem and body image -- particularly those most vulnerable to ...