Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, generate accurate citations for free.

  • Knowledge Base

Methodology

  • How to Write a Strong Hypothesis | Steps & Examples

How to Write a Strong Hypothesis | Steps & Examples

Published on May 6, 2022 by Shona McCombes . Revised on November 20, 2023.

A hypothesis is a statement that can be tested by scientific research. If you want to test a relationship between two or more variables, you need to write hypotheses before you start your experiment or data collection .

Example: Hypothesis

Daily apple consumption leads to fewer doctor’s visits.

Table of contents

What is a hypothesis, developing a hypothesis (with example), hypothesis examples, other interesting articles, frequently asked questions about writing hypotheses.

A hypothesis states your predictions about what your research will find. It is a tentative answer to your research question that has not yet been tested. For some research projects, you might have to write several hypotheses that address different aspects of your research question.

A hypothesis is not just a guess – it should be based on existing theories and knowledge. It also has to be testable, which means you can support or refute it through scientific research methods (such as experiments, observations and statistical analysis of data).

Variables in hypotheses

Hypotheses propose a relationship between two or more types of variables .

  • An independent variable is something the researcher changes or controls.
  • A dependent variable is something the researcher observes and measures.

If there are any control variables , extraneous variables , or confounding variables , be sure to jot those down as you go to minimize the chances that research bias  will affect your results.

In this example, the independent variable is exposure to the sun – the assumed cause . The dependent variable is the level of happiness – the assumed effect .

Here's why students love Scribbr's proofreading services

Discover proofreading & editing

Step 1. Ask a question

Writing a hypothesis begins with a research question that you want to answer. The question should be focused, specific, and researchable within the constraints of your project.

Step 2. Do some preliminary research

Your initial answer to the question should be based on what is already known about the topic. Look for theories and previous studies to help you form educated assumptions about what your research will find.

At this stage, you might construct a conceptual framework to ensure that you’re embarking on a relevant topic . This can also help you identify which variables you will study and what you think the relationships are between them. Sometimes, you’ll have to operationalize more complex constructs.

Step 3. Formulate your hypothesis

Now you should have some idea of what you expect to find. Write your initial answer to the question in a clear, concise sentence.

4. Refine your hypothesis

You need to make sure your hypothesis is specific and testable. There are various ways of phrasing a hypothesis, but all the terms you use should have clear definitions, and the hypothesis should contain:

  • The relevant variables
  • The specific group being studied
  • The predicted outcome of the experiment or analysis

5. Phrase your hypothesis in three ways

To identify the variables, you can write a simple prediction in  if…then form. The first part of the sentence states the independent variable and the second part states the dependent variable.

In academic research, hypotheses are more commonly phrased in terms of correlations or effects, where you directly state the predicted relationship between variables.

If you are comparing two groups, the hypothesis can state what difference you expect to find between them.

6. Write a null hypothesis

If your research involves statistical hypothesis testing , you will also have to write a null hypothesis . The null hypothesis is the default position that there is no association between the variables. The null hypothesis is written as H 0 , while the alternative hypothesis is H 1 or H a .

  • H 0 : The number of lectures attended by first-year students has no effect on their final exam scores.
  • H 1 : The number of lectures attended by first-year students has a positive effect on their final exam scores.

If you want to know more about the research process , methodology , research bias , or statistics , make sure to check out some of our other articles with explanations and examples.

  • Sampling methods
  • Simple random sampling
  • Stratified sampling
  • Cluster sampling
  • Likert scales
  • Reproducibility

 Statistics

  • Null hypothesis
  • Statistical power
  • Probability distribution
  • Effect size
  • Poisson distribution

Research bias

  • Optimism bias
  • Cognitive bias
  • Implicit bias
  • Hawthorne effect
  • Anchoring bias
  • Explicit bias

Prevent plagiarism. Run a free check.

A hypothesis is not just a guess — it should be based on existing theories and knowledge. It also has to be testable, which means you can support or refute it through scientific research methods (such as experiments, observations and statistical analysis of data).

Null and alternative hypotheses are used in statistical hypothesis testing . The null hypothesis of a test always predicts no effect or no relationship between variables, while the alternative hypothesis states your research prediction of an effect or relationship.

Hypothesis testing is a formal procedure for investigating our ideas about the world using statistics. It is used by scientists to test specific predictions, called hypotheses , by calculating how likely it is that a pattern or relationship between variables could have arisen by chance.

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the “Cite this Scribbr article” button to automatically add the citation to our free Citation Generator.

McCombes, S. (2023, November 20). How to Write a Strong Hypothesis | Steps & Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved April 2, 2024, from https://www.scribbr.com/methodology/hypothesis/

Is this article helpful?

Shona McCombes

Shona McCombes

Other students also liked, construct validity | definition, types, & examples, what is a conceptual framework | tips & examples, operationalization | a guide with examples, pros & cons, unlimited academic ai-proofreading.

✔ Document error-free in 5minutes ✔ Unlimited document corrections ✔ Specialized in correcting academic texts

Our websites may use cookies to personalize and enhance your experience. By continuing without changing your cookie settings, you agree to this collection. For more information, please see our University Websites Privacy Notice .

Neag School of Education

Educational Research Basics by Del Siegle

Null and alternative hypotheses.

Converting research questions to hypothesis is a simple task. Take the questions and make it a positive statement that says a relationship exists (correlation studies) or a difference exists between the groups (experiment study) and you have the alternative hypothesis. Write the statement such that a relationship does not exist or a difference does not exist and you have the null hypothesis. You can reverse the process if you have a hypothesis and wish to write a research question.

When you are comparing two groups, the groups are the independent variable. When you are testing whether something affects something else, the cause is the independent variable. The independent variable is the one you manipulate.

Teachers given higher pay will have more positive attitudes toward children than teachers given lower pay. The first step is to ask yourself “Are there two or more groups being compared?” The answer is “Yes.” What are the groups? Teachers who are given higher pay and teachers who are given lower pay. The independent variable is teacher pay. The dependent variable (the outcome) is attitude towards school.

You could also approach is another way. “Is something causing something else?” The answer is “Yes.”  What is causing what? Teacher pay is causing attitude towards school. Therefore, teacher pay is the independent variable (cause) and attitude towards school is the dependent variable (outcome).

By tradition, we try to disprove (reject) the null hypothesis. We can never prove a null hypothesis, because it is impossible to prove something does not exist. We can disprove something does not exist by finding an example of it. Therefore, in research we try to disprove the null hypothesis. When we do find that a relationship (or difference) exists then we reject the null and accept the alternative. If we do not find that a relationship (or difference) exists, we fail to reject the null hypothesis (and go with it). We never say we accept the null hypothesis because it is never possible to prove something does not exist. That is why we say that we failed to reject the null hypothesis, rather than we accepted it.

Del Siegle, Ph.D. Neag School of Education – University of Connecticut [email protected] www.delsiegle.com

  • Academic Skills
  • Graduate research services
  • Writing thesis sections - Part 2

Shaping the research question and hypothesis

This resource introduces approaches to writing the core parts of your thesis. It includes activities to help you apply tips and reflect on your own learning, and should take you 15-20 minutes to read and complete. Check out the further resources at the bottom of each section and references on the last page for more information on this topic.

The core part of your thesis starts with your research question or hypothesis and proceeds through your explanation of methods and results, or data analysis, and ends with a discussion of your findings.

The research question and hypothesis mark your own disciplinary territory and drive your research.

Whether you have both a research question and a hypothesis, and whether they are broken down into several sub-areas, will depend on your discipline and topic; but however they appear, it is important that they are clearly formulated.

Research question

A research question should:

  • Focus on a clear, specific and significant problem or puzzle
  • Be shaped in a way that allows it to be answered with different research results
  • Be revisited frequently in the research process.

You can use the FINER criteria when developing a research question:

  • F easible – is the research practical and achievable given time and resources?
  • I nteresting – is the research intriguing, compelling and able to contribute to the field?
  • N ovel – is the research original?
  • E thical – are there any concerns about the impact of the research on participants or wider community?
  • R elevant – is the research important or meaningful for those in the field?

Example research questions:

  • What impact will climate change have on production systems in the Tasmanian dairy industry?
  • What are the unique challenges and opportunities for higher education institutions that allow first-year undergraduate students to use Generative AI tools in written assessments?

A hypothesis is a well-reasoned proposition in response to a research question that you will test to confirm or disprove in your research. Not all research has a hypothesis. A hypothesis should:

  • Be in the form of a statement
  • Be disprovable
  • Be clear and specific in scope.

Example hypothesis:

  • Undergraduate students who participate in a stress-reduction seminar will report an increase in their well-being after six weeks This is a statement that argues for a relationship between two clear and specific variables that can be tested and thus disproven.

Types of research questions

In order to help you frame a research question, keep in mind that there are four basic types of research (adapted from Fahnestock & Secor, 2004).

Four general research types and their corresponding  questions:

Note: almost all graduate research will involve elements of each of these types. However, it is useful to focus primarily on one of these questions.

Contextualise the research question or hypothesis

The research question or hypothesis is part of your thesis core as it guides your own research, but it is usually stated in a lead-in section, such as the thesis introduction.

Complete the activity below to learn how a research question or hypothesis can be contextualised.

*If content below does not display, please refresh your browser

Note that some disciplines require hypotheses rather than research questions and vice versa. However, the question implied in this example seems to be ‘What role does fast food play in the childhood obesity problem in the province?’

FINER: A research framework

What is and how to write a good hypothesis in research?

Use the side menu to go the next section: Choosing and justifying your methods , where we look at ways to present your research design.

Logo for Pressbooks

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

Overview of the Scientific Method

Learning Objectives

  • Distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis.
  • Discover how theories are used to generate hypotheses and how the results of studies can be used to further inform theories.
  • Understand the characteristics of a good hypothesis.

Theories and Hypotheses

Before describing how to develop a hypothesis, it is important to distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis. A  theory  is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena. Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes, functions, or organizing principles that have not been observed directly. Consider, for example, Zajonc’s theory of social facilitation and social inhibition (1965) [1] . He proposed that being watched by others while performing a task creates a general state of physiological arousal, which increases the likelihood of the dominant (most likely) response. So for highly practiced tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make correct responses, but for relatively unpracticed tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make incorrect responses. Notice that this theory—which has come to be called drive theory—provides an explanation of both social facilitation and social inhibition that goes beyond the phenomena themselves by including concepts such as “arousal” and “dominant response,” along with processes such as the effect of arousal on the dominant response.

Outside of science, referring to an idea as a theory often implies that it is untested—perhaps no more than a wild guess. In science, however, the term theory has no such implication. A theory is simply an explanation or interpretation of a set of phenomena. It can be untested, but it can also be extensively tested, well supported, and accepted as an accurate description of the world by the scientific community. The theory of evolution by natural selection, for example, is a theory because it is an explanation of the diversity of life on earth—not because it is untested or unsupported by scientific research. On the contrary, the evidence for this theory is overwhelmingly positive and nearly all scientists accept its basic assumptions as accurate. Similarly, the “germ theory” of disease is a theory because it is an explanation of the origin of various diseases, not because there is any doubt that many diseases are caused by microorganisms that infect the body.

A  hypothesis , on the other hand, is a specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate. It is an explanation that relies on just a few key concepts. Hypotheses are often specific predictions about what will happen in a particular study. They are developed by considering existing evidence and using reasoning to infer what will happen in the specific context of interest. Hypotheses are often but not always derived from theories. So a hypothesis is often a prediction based on a theory but some hypotheses are a-theoretical and only after a set of observations have been made, is a theory developed. This is because theories are broad in nature and they explain larger bodies of data. So if our research question is really original then we may need to collect some data and make some observations before we can develop a broader theory.

Theories and hypotheses always have this  if-then  relationship. “ If   drive theory is correct,  then  cockroaches should run through a straight runway faster, and a branching runway more slowly, when other cockroaches are present.” Although hypotheses are usually expressed as statements, they can always be rephrased as questions. “Do cockroaches run through a straight runway faster when other cockroaches are present?” Thus deriving hypotheses from theories is an excellent way of generating interesting research questions.

But how do researchers derive hypotheses from theories? One way is to generate a research question using the techniques discussed in this chapter  and then ask whether any theory implies an answer to that question. For example, you might wonder whether expressive writing about positive experiences improves health as much as expressive writing about traumatic experiences. Although this  question  is an interesting one  on its own, you might then ask whether the habituation theory—the idea that expressive writing causes people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings—implies an answer. In this case, it seems clear that if the habituation theory is correct, then expressive writing about positive experiences should not be effective because it would not cause people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings. A second way to derive hypotheses from theories is to focus on some component of the theory that has not yet been directly observed. For example, a researcher could focus on the process of habituation—perhaps hypothesizing that people should show fewer signs of emotional distress with each new writing session.

Among the very best hypotheses are those that distinguish between competing theories. For example, Norbert Schwarz and his colleagues considered two theories of how people make judgments about themselves, such as how assertive they are (Schwarz et al., 1991) [2] . Both theories held that such judgments are based on relevant examples that people bring to mind. However, one theory was that people base their judgments on the  number  of examples they bring to mind and the other was that people base their judgments on how  easily  they bring those examples to mind. To test these theories, the researchers asked people to recall either six times when they were assertive (which is easy for most people) or 12 times (which is difficult for most people). Then they asked them to judge their own assertiveness. Note that the number-of-examples theory implies that people who recalled 12 examples should judge themselves to be more assertive because they recalled more examples, but the ease-of-examples theory implies that participants who recalled six examples should judge themselves as more assertive because recalling the examples was easier. Thus the two theories made opposite predictions so that only one of the predictions could be confirmed. The surprising result was that participants who recalled fewer examples judged themselves to be more assertive—providing particularly convincing evidence in favor of the ease-of-retrieval theory over the number-of-examples theory.

Theory Testing

The primary way that scientific researchers use theories is sometimes called the hypothetico-deductive method  (although this term is much more likely to be used by philosophers of science than by scientists themselves). Researchers begin with a set of phenomena and either construct a theory to explain or interpret them or choose an existing theory to work with. They then make a prediction about some new phenomenon that should be observed if the theory is correct. Again, this prediction is called a hypothesis. The researchers then conduct an empirical study to test the hypothesis. Finally, they reevaluate the theory in light of the new results and revise it if necessary. This process is usually conceptualized as a cycle because the researchers can then derive a new hypothesis from the revised theory, conduct a new empirical study to test the hypothesis, and so on. As  Figure 2.3  shows, this approach meshes nicely with the model of scientific research in psychology presented earlier in the textbook—creating a more detailed model of “theoretically motivated” or “theory-driven” research.

converting research questions into hypothesis

As an example, let us consider Zajonc’s research on social facilitation and inhibition. He started with a somewhat contradictory pattern of results from the research literature. He then constructed his drive theory, according to which being watched by others while performing a task causes physiological arousal, which increases an organism’s tendency to make the dominant response. This theory predicts social facilitation for well-learned tasks and social inhibition for poorly learned tasks. He now had a theory that organized previous results in a meaningful way—but he still needed to test it. He hypothesized that if his theory was correct, he should observe that the presence of others improves performance in a simple laboratory task but inhibits performance in a difficult version of the very same laboratory task. To test this hypothesis, one of the studies he conducted used cockroaches as subjects (Zajonc, Heingartner, & Herman, 1969) [3] . The cockroaches ran either down a straight runway (an easy task for a cockroach) or through a cross-shaped maze (a difficult task for a cockroach) to escape into a dark chamber when a light was shined on them. They did this either while alone or in the presence of other cockroaches in clear plastic “audience boxes.” Zajonc found that cockroaches in the straight runway reached their goal more quickly in the presence of other cockroaches, but cockroaches in the cross-shaped maze reached their goal more slowly when they were in the presence of other cockroaches. Thus he confirmed his hypothesis and provided support for his drive theory. (Zajonc also showed that drive theory existed in humans [Zajonc & Sales, 1966] [4] in many other studies afterward).

Incorporating Theory into Your Research

When you write your research report or plan your presentation, be aware that there are two basic ways that researchers usually include theory. The first is to raise a research question, answer that question by conducting a new study, and then offer one or more theories (usually more) to explain or interpret the results. This format works well for applied research questions and for research questions that existing theories do not address. The second way is to describe one or more existing theories, derive a hypothesis from one of those theories, test the hypothesis in a new study, and finally reevaluate the theory. This format works well when there is an existing theory that addresses the research question—especially if the resulting hypothesis is surprising or conflicts with a hypothesis derived from a different theory.

To use theories in your research will not only give you guidance in coming up with experiment ideas and possible projects, but it lends legitimacy to your work. Psychologists have been interested in a variety of human behaviors and have developed many theories along the way. Using established theories will help you break new ground as a researcher, not limit you from developing your own ideas.

Characteristics of a Good Hypothesis

There are three general characteristics of a good hypothesis. First, a good hypothesis must be testable and falsifiable . We must be able to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and if you’ll recall Popper’s falsifiability criterion, it must be possible to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false. Second, a good hypothesis must be logical. As described above, hypotheses are more than just a random guess. Hypotheses should be informed by previous theories or observations and logical reasoning. Typically, we begin with a broad and general theory and use  deductive reasoning to generate a more specific hypothesis to test based on that theory. Occasionally, however, when there is no theory to inform our hypothesis, we use  inductive reasoning  which involves using specific observations or research findings to form a more general hypothesis. Finally, the hypothesis should be positive. That is, the hypothesis should make a positive statement about the existence of a relationship or effect, rather than a statement that a relationship or effect does not exist. As scientists, we don’t set out to show that relationships do not exist or that effects do not occur so our hypotheses should not be worded in a way to suggest that an effect or relationship does not exist. The nature of science is to assume that something does not exist and then seek to find evidence to prove this wrong, to show that it really does exist. That may seem backward to you but that is the nature of the scientific method. The underlying reason for this is beyond the scope of this chapter but it has to do with statistical theory.

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation.  Science, 149 , 269–274 ↵
  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 195–202. ↵
  • Zajonc, R. B., Heingartner, A., & Herman, E. M. (1969). Social enhancement and impairment of performance in the cockroach.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13 , 83–92. ↵
  • Zajonc, R.B. & Sales, S.M. (1966). Social facilitation of dominant and subordinate responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 , 160-168. ↵

A coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena.

A specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate.

A cyclical process of theory development, starting with an observed phenomenon, then developing or using a theory to make a specific prediction of what should happen if that theory is correct, testing that prediction, refining the theory in light of the findings, and using that refined theory to develop new hypotheses, and so on.

The ability to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and the possibility to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false.

Research Methods in Psychology Copyright © 2019 by Rajiv S. Jhangiani, I-Chant A. Chiang, Carrie Cuttler, & Dana C. Leighton is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Hypothesis Maker Online

Looking for a hypothesis maker? This online tool for students will help you formulate a beautiful hypothesis quickly, efficiently, and for free.

Are you looking for an effective hypothesis maker online? Worry no more; try our online tool for students and formulate your hypothesis within no time.

  • 🔎 How to Use the Tool?
  • ⚗️ What Is a Hypothesis in Science?

👍 What Does a Good Hypothesis Mean?

  • 🧭 Steps to Making a Good Hypothesis

🔗 References

📄 hypothesis maker: how to use it.

Our hypothesis maker is a simple and efficient tool you can access online for free.

If you want to create a research hypothesis quickly, you should fill out the research details in the given fields on the hypothesis generator.

Below are the fields you should complete to generate your hypothesis:

  • Who or what is your research based on? For instance, the subject can be research group 1.
  • What does the subject (research group 1) do?
  • What does the subject affect? - This shows the predicted outcome, which is the object.
  • Who or what will be compared with research group 1? (research group 2).

Once you fill the in the fields, you can click the ‘Make a hypothesis’ tab and get your results.

⚗️ What Is a Hypothesis in the Scientific Method?

A hypothesis is a statement describing an expectation or prediction of your research through observation.

It is similar to academic speculation and reasoning that discloses the outcome of your scientific test . An effective hypothesis, therefore, should be crafted carefully and with precision.

A good hypothesis should have dependent and independent variables . These variables are the elements you will test in your research method – it can be a concept, an event, or an object as long as it is observable.

You can observe the dependent variables while the independent variables keep changing during the experiment.

In a nutshell, a hypothesis directs and organizes the research methods you will use, forming a large section of research paper writing.

Hypothesis vs. Theory

A hypothesis is a realistic expectation that researchers make before any investigation. It is formulated and tested to prove whether the statement is true. A theory, on the other hand, is a factual principle supported by evidence. Thus, a theory is more fact-backed compared to a hypothesis.

Another difference is that a hypothesis is presented as a single statement , while a theory can be an assortment of things . Hypotheses are based on future possibilities toward a specific projection, but the results are uncertain. Theories are verified with undisputable results because of proper substantiation.

When it comes to data, a hypothesis relies on limited information , while a theory is established on an extensive data set tested on various conditions.

You should observe the stated assumption to prove its accuracy.

Since hypotheses have observable variables, their outcome is usually based on a specific occurrence. Conversely, theories are grounded on a general principle involving multiple experiments and research tests.

This general principle can apply to many specific cases.

The primary purpose of formulating a hypothesis is to present a tentative prediction for researchers to explore further through tests and observations. Theories, in their turn, aim to explain plausible occurrences in the form of a scientific study.

It would help to rely on several criteria to establish a good hypothesis. Below are the parameters you should use to analyze the quality of your hypothesis.

🧭 6 Steps to Making a Good Hypothesis

Writing a hypothesis becomes way simpler if you follow a tried-and-tested algorithm. Let’s explore how you can formulate a good hypothesis in a few steps:

Step #1: Ask Questions

The first step in hypothesis creation is asking real questions about the surrounding reality.

Why do things happen as they do? What are the causes of some occurrences?

Your curiosity will trigger great questions that you can use to formulate a stellar hypothesis. So, ensure you pick a research topic of interest to scrutinize the world’s phenomena, processes, and events.

Step #2: Do Initial Research

Carry out preliminary research and gather essential background information about your topic of choice.

The extent of the information you collect will depend on what you want to prove.

Your initial research can be complete with a few academic books or a simple Internet search for quick answers with relevant statistics.

Still, keep in mind that in this phase, it is too early to prove or disapprove of your hypothesis.

Step #3: Identify Your Variables

Now that you have a basic understanding of the topic, choose the dependent and independent variables.

Take note that independent variables are the ones you can’t control, so understand the limitations of your test before settling on a final hypothesis.

Step #4: Formulate Your Hypothesis

You can write your hypothesis as an ‘if – then’ expression . Presenting any hypothesis in this format is reliable since it describes the cause-and-effect you want to test.

For instance: If I study every day, then I will get good grades.

Step #5: Gather Relevant Data

Once you have identified your variables and formulated the hypothesis, you can start the experiment. Remember, the conclusion you make will be a proof or rebuttal of your initial assumption.

So, gather relevant information, whether for a simple or statistical hypothesis, because you need to back your statement.

Step #6: Record Your Findings

Finally, write down your conclusions in a research paper .

Outline in detail whether the test has proved or disproved your hypothesis.

Edit and proofread your work, using a plagiarism checker to ensure the authenticity of your text.

We hope that the above tips will be useful for you. Note that if you need to conduct business analysis, you can use the free templates we’ve prepared: SWOT , PESTLE , VRIO , SOAR , and Porter’s 5 Forces .

❓ Hypothesis Formulator FAQ

Updated: Oct 25th, 2023

  • How to Write a Hypothesis in 6 Steps - Grammarly
  • Forming a Good Hypothesis for Scientific Research
  • The Hypothesis in Science Writing
  • Scientific Method: Step 3: HYPOTHESIS - Subject Guides
  • Hypothesis Template & Examples - Video & Lesson Transcript
  • Free Essays
  • Writing Tools
  • Lit. Guides
  • Donate a Paper
  • Referencing Guides
  • Free Textbooks
  • Tongue Twisters
  • Job Openings
  • Expert Application
  • Video Contest
  • Writing Scholarship
  • Discount Codes
  • IvyPanda Shop
  • Terms and Conditions
  • Privacy Policy
  • Cookies Policy
  • Copyright Principles
  • DMCA Request
  • Service Notice

Use our hypothesis maker whenever you need to formulate a hypothesis for your study. We offer a very simple tool where you just need to provide basic info about your variables, subjects, and predicted outcomes. The rest is on us. Get a perfect hypothesis in no time!

Logo for Portland State University Pressbooks

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

Developing a Hypothesis

Rajiv S. Jhangiani; I-Chant A. Chiang; Carrie Cuttler; and Dana C. Leighton

Learning Objectives

  • Distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis.
  • Discover how theories are used to generate hypotheses and how the results of studies can be used to further inform theories.
  • Understand the characteristics of a good hypothesis.

Theories and Hypotheses

Before describing how to develop a hypothesis, it is important to distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis. A  theory  is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena. Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes, functions, or organizing principles that have not been observed directly. Consider, for example, Zajonc’s theory of social facilitation and social inhibition (1965) [1] . He proposed that being watched by others while performing a task creates a general state of physiological arousal, which increases the likelihood of the dominant (most likely) response. So for highly practiced tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make correct responses, but for relatively unpracticed tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make incorrect responses. Notice that this theory—which has come to be called drive theory—provides an explanation of both social facilitation and social inhibition that goes beyond the phenomena themselves by including concepts such as “arousal” and “dominant response,” along with processes such as the effect of arousal on the dominant response.

Outside of science, referring to an idea as a theory often implies that it is untested—perhaps no more than a wild guess. In science, however, the term theory has no such implication. A theory is simply an explanation or interpretation of a set of phenomena. It can be untested, but it can also be extensively tested, well supported, and accepted as an accurate description of the world by the scientific community. The theory of evolution by natural selection, for example, is a theory because it is an explanation of the diversity of life on earth—not because it is untested or unsupported by scientific research. On the contrary, the evidence for this theory is overwhelmingly positive and nearly all scientists accept its basic assumptions as accurate. Similarly, the “germ theory” of disease is a theory because it is an explanation of the origin of various diseases, not because there is any doubt that many diseases are caused by microorganisms that infect the body.

A  hypothesis , on the other hand, is a specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate. It is an explanation that relies on just a few key concepts. Hypotheses are often specific predictions about what will happen in a particular study. They are developed by considering existing evidence and using reasoning to infer what will happen in the specific context of interest. Hypotheses are often but not always derived from theories. So a hypothesis is often a prediction based on a theory but some hypotheses are a-theoretical and only after a set of observations have been made, is a theory developed. This is because theories are broad in nature and they explain larger bodies of data. So if our research question is really original then we may need to collect some data and make some observations before we can develop a broader theory.

Theories and hypotheses always have this  if-then  relationship. “ If   drive theory is correct,  then  cockroaches should run through a straight runway faster, and a branching runway more slowly, when other cockroaches are present.” Although hypotheses are usually expressed as statements, they can always be rephrased as questions. “Do cockroaches run through a straight runway faster when other cockroaches are present?” Thus deriving hypotheses from theories is an excellent way of generating interesting research questions.

But how do researchers derive hypotheses from theories? One way is to generate a research question using the techniques discussed in this chapter  and then ask whether any theory implies an answer to that question. For example, you might wonder whether expressive writing about positive experiences improves health as much as expressive writing about traumatic experiences. Although this  question  is an interesting one  on its own, you might then ask whether the habituation theory—the idea that expressive writing causes people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings—implies an answer. In this case, it seems clear that if the habituation theory is correct, then expressive writing about positive experiences should not be effective because it would not cause people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings. A second way to derive hypotheses from theories is to focus on some component of the theory that has not yet been directly observed. For example, a researcher could focus on the process of habituation—perhaps hypothesizing that people should show fewer signs of emotional distress with each new writing session.

Among the very best hypotheses are those that distinguish between competing theories. For example, Norbert Schwarz and his colleagues considered two theories of how people make judgments about themselves, such as how assertive they are (Schwarz et al., 1991) [2] . Both theories held that such judgments are based on relevant examples that people bring to mind. However, one theory was that people base their judgments on the  number  of examples they bring to mind and the other was that people base their judgments on how  easily  they bring those examples to mind. To test these theories, the researchers asked people to recall either six times when they were assertive (which is easy for most people) or 12 times (which is difficult for most people). Then they asked them to judge their own assertiveness. Note that the number-of-examples theory implies that people who recalled 12 examples should judge themselves to be more assertive because they recalled more examples, but the ease-of-examples theory implies that participants who recalled six examples should judge themselves as more assertive because recalling the examples was easier. Thus the two theories made opposite predictions so that only one of the predictions could be confirmed. The surprising result was that participants who recalled fewer examples judged themselves to be more assertive—providing particularly convincing evidence in favor of the ease-of-retrieval theory over the number-of-examples theory.

Theory Testing

The primary way that scientific researchers use theories is sometimes called the hypothetico-deductive method  (although this term is much more likely to be used by philosophers of science than by scientists themselves). Researchers begin with a set of phenomena and either construct a theory to explain or interpret them or choose an existing theory to work with. They then make a prediction about some new phenomenon that should be observed if the theory is correct. Again, this prediction is called a hypothesis. The researchers then conduct an empirical study to test the hypothesis. Finally, they reevaluate the theory in light of the new results and revise it if necessary. This process is usually conceptualized as a cycle because the researchers can then derive a new hypothesis from the revised theory, conduct a new empirical study to test the hypothesis, and so on. As  Figure 2.3  shows, this approach meshes nicely with the model of scientific research in psychology presented earlier in the textbook—creating a more detailed model of “theoretically motivated” or “theory-driven” research.

converting research questions into hypothesis

As an example, let us consider Zajonc’s research on social facilitation and inhibition. He started with a somewhat contradictory pattern of results from the research literature. He then constructed his drive theory, according to which being watched by others while performing a task causes physiological arousal, which increases an organism’s tendency to make the dominant response. This theory predicts social facilitation for well-learned tasks and social inhibition for poorly learned tasks. He now had a theory that organized previous results in a meaningful way—but he still needed to test it. He hypothesized that if his theory was correct, he should observe that the presence of others improves performance in a simple laboratory task but inhibits performance in a difficult version of the very same laboratory task. To test this hypothesis, one of the studies he conducted used cockroaches as subjects (Zajonc, Heingartner, & Herman, 1969) [3] . The cockroaches ran either down a straight runway (an easy task for a cockroach) or through a cross-shaped maze (a difficult task for a cockroach) to escape into a dark chamber when a light was shined on them. They did this either while alone or in the presence of other cockroaches in clear plastic “audience boxes.” Zajonc found that cockroaches in the straight runway reached their goal more quickly in the presence of other cockroaches, but cockroaches in the cross-shaped maze reached their goal more slowly when they were in the presence of other cockroaches. Thus he confirmed his hypothesis and provided support for his drive theory. (Zajonc also showed that drive theory existed in humans [Zajonc & Sales, 1966] [4] in many other studies afterward).

Incorporating Theory into Your Research

When you write your research report or plan your presentation, be aware that there are two basic ways that researchers usually include theory. The first is to raise a research question, answer that question by conducting a new study, and then offer one or more theories (usually more) to explain or interpret the results. This format works well for applied research questions and for research questions that existing theories do not address. The second way is to describe one or more existing theories, derive a hypothesis from one of those theories, test the hypothesis in a new study, and finally reevaluate the theory. This format works well when there is an existing theory that addresses the research question—especially if the resulting hypothesis is surprising or conflicts with a hypothesis derived from a different theory.

To use theories in your research will not only give you guidance in coming up with experiment ideas and possible projects, but it lends legitimacy to your work. Psychologists have been interested in a variety of human behaviors and have developed many theories along the way. Using established theories will help you break new ground as a researcher, not limit you from developing your own ideas.

Characteristics of a Good Hypothesis

There are three general characteristics of a good hypothesis. First, a good hypothesis must be testable and falsifiable . We must be able to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and if you’ll recall Popper’s falsifiability criterion, it must be possible to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false. Second, a good hypothesis must be logical. As described above, hypotheses are more than just a random guess. Hypotheses should be informed by previous theories or observations and logical reasoning. Typically, we begin with a broad and general theory and use  deductive reasoning to generate a more specific hypothesis to test based on that theory. Occasionally, however, when there is no theory to inform our hypothesis, we use  inductive reasoning  which involves using specific observations or research findings to form a more general hypothesis. Finally, the hypothesis should be positive. That is, the hypothesis should make a positive statement about the existence of a relationship or effect, rather than a statement that a relationship or effect does not exist. As scientists, we don’t set out to show that relationships do not exist or that effects do not occur so our hypotheses should not be worded in a way to suggest that an effect or relationship does not exist. The nature of science is to assume that something does not exist and then seek to find evidence to prove this wrong, to show that it really does exist. That may seem backward to you but that is the nature of the scientific method. The underlying reason for this is beyond the scope of this chapter but it has to do with statistical theory.

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation.  Science, 149 , 269–274 ↵
  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 195–202. ↵
  • Zajonc, R. B., Heingartner, A., & Herman, E. M. (1969). Social enhancement and impairment of performance in the cockroach.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13 , 83–92. ↵
  • Zajonc, R.B. & Sales, S.M. (1966). Social facilitation of dominant and subordinate responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 , 160-168. ↵

A coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena.

A specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate.

A cyclical process of theory development, starting with an observed phenomenon, then developing or using a theory to make a specific prediction of what should happen if that theory is correct, testing that prediction, refining the theory in light of the findings, and using that refined theory to develop new hypotheses, and so on.

The ability to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and the possibility to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false.

Developing a Hypothesis Copyright © 2022 by Rajiv S. Jhangiani; I-Chant A. Chiang; Carrie Cuttler; and Dana C. Leighton is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Learn How To Write A Hypothesis For Your Next Research Project!

blog image

Undoubtedly, research plays a crucial role in substantiating or refuting our assumptions. These assumptions act as potential answers to our questions. Such assumptions, also known as hypotheses, are considered key aspects of research. In this blog, we delve into the significance of hypotheses. And provide insights on how to write them effectively. So, let’s dive in and explore the art of writing hypotheses together.

Table of Contents

What is a Hypothesis?

A hypothesis is a crucial starting point in scientific research. It is an educated guess about the relationship between two or more variables. In other words, a hypothesis acts as a foundation for a researcher to build their study.

Here are some examples of well-crafted hypotheses:

  • Increased exposure to natural sunlight improves sleep quality in adults.

A positive relationship between natural sunlight exposure and sleep quality in adult individuals.

  • Playing puzzle games on a regular basis enhances problem-solving abilities in children.

Engaging in frequent puzzle gameplay leads to improved problem-solving skills in children.

  • Students and improved learning hecks.

S tudents using online  paper writing service  platforms (as a learning tool for receiving personalized feedback and guidance) will demonstrate improved writing skills. (compared to those who do not utilize such platforms).

  • The use of APA format in research papers. 

Using the  APA format  helps students stay organized when writing research papers. Organized students can focus better on their topics and, as a result, produce better quality work.

The Building Blocks of a Hypothesis

To better understand the concept of a hypothesis, let’s break it down into its basic components:

  • Variables . A hypothesis involves at least two variables. An independent variable and a dependent variable. The independent variable is the one being changed or manipulated, while the dependent variable is the one being measured or observed.
  • Relationship : A hypothesis proposes a relationship or connection between the variables. This could be a cause-and-effect relationship or a correlation between them.
  • Testability : A hypothesis should be testable and falsifiable, meaning it can be proven right or wrong through experimentation or observation.

Types of Hypotheses

When learning how to write a hypothesis, it’s essential to understand its main types. These include; alternative hypotheses and null hypotheses. In the following section, we explore both types of hypotheses with examples. 

Alternative Hypothesis (H1)

This kind of hypothesis suggests a relationship or effect between the variables. It is the main focus of the study. The researcher wants to either prove or disprove it. Many research divides this hypothesis into two subsections: 

  • Directional 

This type of H1 predicts a specific outcome. Many researchers use this hypothesis to explore the relationship between variables rather than the groups. 

  • Non-directional

You can take a guess from the name. This type of H1 does not provide a specific prediction for the research outcome. 

Here are some examples for your better understanding of how to write a hypothesis.

  • Consuming caffeine improves cognitive performance.  (This hypothesis predicts that there is a positive relationship between caffeine consumption and cognitive performance.)
  • Aerobic exercise leads to reduced blood pressure.  (This hypothesis suggests that engaging in aerobic exercise results in lower blood pressure readings.)
  • Exposure to nature reduces stress levels among employees.  (Here, the hypothesis proposes that employees exposed to natural environments will experience decreased stress levels.)
  • Listening to classical music while studying increases memory retention.  (This hypothesis speculates that studying with classical music playing in the background boosts students’ ability to retain information.)
  • Early literacy intervention improves reading skills in children.  (This hypothesis claims that providing early literacy assistance to children results in enhanced reading abilities.)
  • Time management in nursing students. ( Students who use a  nursing research paper writing service  have more time to focus on their studies and can achieve better grades in other subjects. )

Null Hypothesis (H0)

A null hypothesis assumes no relationship or effect between the variables. If the alternative hypothesis is proven to be false, the null hypothesis is considered to be true. Usually a null hypothesis shows no direct correlation between the defined variables. 

Here are some of the examples

  • The consumption of herbal tea has no effect on sleep quality.  (This hypothesis assumes that herbal tea consumption does not impact the quality of sleep.)
  • The number of hours spent playing video games is unrelated to academic performance.  (Here, the null hypothesis suggests that no relationship exists between video gameplay duration and academic achievement.)
  • Implementing flexible work schedules has no influence on employee job satisfaction.  (This hypothesis contends that providing flexible schedules does not affect how satisfied employees are with their jobs.)
  • Writing ability of a 7th grader is not affected by reading editorial example. ( There is no relationship between reading an  editorial example  and improving a 7th grader’s writing abilities.) 
  • The type of lighting in a room does not affect people’s mood.  (In this null hypothesis, there is no connection between the kind of lighting in a room and the mood of those present.)
  • The use of social media during break time does not impact productivity at work.  (This hypothesis proposes that social media usage during breaks has no effect on work productivity.)

As you learn how to write a hypothesis, remember that aiming for clarity, testability, and relevance to your research question is vital. By mastering this skill, you’re well on your way to conducting impactful scientific research. Good luck!

Importance of a Hypothesis in Research

A well-structured hypothesis is a vital part of any research project for several reasons:

  • It provides clear direction for the study by setting its focus and purpose.
  • It outlines expectations of the research, making it easier to measure results.
  • It helps identify any potential limitations in the study, allowing researchers to refine their approach.

In conclusion, a hypothesis plays a fundamental role in the research process. By understanding its concept and constructing a well-thought-out hypothesis, researchers lay the groundwork for a successful, scientifically sound investigation.

How to Write a Hypothesis?

Here are five steps that you can follow to write an effective hypothesis. 

Step 1: Identify Your Research Question

The first step in learning how to compose a hypothesis is to clearly define your research question. This question is the central focus of your study and will help you determine the direction of your hypothesis.

Step 2: Determine the Variables

When exploring how to write a hypothesis, it’s crucial to identify the variables involved in your study. You’ll need at least two variables:

  • Independent variable : The factor you manipulate or change in your experiment.
  • Dependent variable : The outcome or result you observe or measure, which is influenced by the independent variable.

Step 3: Build the Hypothetical Relationship

In understanding how to compose a hypothesis, constructing the relationship between the variables is key. Based on your research question and variables, predict the expected outcome or connection. This prediction should be specific, testable, and, if possible, expressed in the “If…then” format.

Step 4: Write the Null Hypothesis

When mastering how to write a hypothesis, it’s important to create a null hypothesis as well. The null hypothesis assumes no relationship or effect between the variables, acting as a counterpoint to your primary hypothesis.

Step 5: Review Your Hypothesis

Finally, when learning how to compose a hypothesis, it’s essential to review your hypothesis for clarity, testability, and relevance to your research question. Make any necessary adjustments to ensure it provides a solid basis for your study.

In conclusion, understanding how to write a hypothesis is crucial for conducting successful scientific research. By focusing on your research question and carefully building relationships between variables, you will lay a strong foundation for advancing research and knowledge in your field.

Hypothesis vs. Prediction: What’s the Difference?

Understanding the differences between a hypothesis and a prediction is crucial in scientific research. Often, these terms are used interchangeably, but they have distinct meanings and functions. This segment aims to clarify these differences and explain how to compose a hypothesis correctly, helping you improve the quality of your research projects.

Hypothesis: The Foundation of Your Research

A hypothesis is an educated guess about the relationship between two or more variables. It provides the basis for your research question and is a starting point for an experiment or observational study.

The critical elements for a hypothesis include:

  • Specificity: A clear and concise statement that describes the relationship between variables.
  • Testability: The ability to test the hypothesis through experimentation or observation.

To learn how to write a hypothesis, it’s essential to identify your research question first and then predict the relationship between the variables.

Prediction: The Expected Outcome

A prediction is a statement about a specific outcome you expect to see in your experiment or observational study. It’s derived from the hypothesis and provides a measurable way to test the relationship between variables.

Here’s an example of how to write a hypothesis and a related prediction:

  • Hypothesis: Consuming a high-sugar diet leads to weight gain.
  • Prediction: People who consume a high-sugar diet for six weeks will gain more weight than those who maintain a low-sugar diet during the same period.

Key Differences Between a Hypothesis and a Prediction

While a hypothesis and prediction are both essential components of scientific research, there are some key differences to keep in mind:

  • A hypothesis is an educated guess that suggests a relationship between variables, while a prediction is a specific and measurable outcome based on that hypothesis.
  • A hypothesis can give rise to multiple experiment or observational study predictions.

To conclude, understanding the differences between a hypothesis and a prediction, and learning how to write a hypothesis, are essential steps to form a robust foundation for your research. By creating clear, testable hypotheses along with specific, measurable predictions, you lay the groundwork for scientifically sound investigations.

Here’s a wrap-up for this guide on how to write a hypothesis. We’re confident this article was helpful for many of you. We understand that many students struggle with writing their school research . However, we hope to continue assisting you through our blog tutorial on writing different aspects of academic assignments.

For further information, you can check out our reverent blog or contact our professionals to avail amazing writing services. Paper perk experts tailor assignments to reflect your unique voice and perspectives. Our professionals make sure to stick around till your satisfaction. So what are you waiting for? Pick your required service and order away!

Order Original Papers & Essays

Your First Custom Paper Sample is on Us!

timely deliveries

Timely Deliveries

premium quality

No Plagiarism & AI

unlimited revisions

100% Refund

Try Our Free Paper Writing Service

Related blogs.

blog-img

Connections with Writers and support

safe service

Privacy and Confidentiality Guarantee

quality-score

Average Quality Score

Use Our Free A/B Testing Hypothesis Generator . Never Miss Key Elements From Your Hypotheses. Get Big Conversion Lifts.

Observation, inadvertent impact.

Get Toolkit

Streamline Your Hypothesis Generation Research with Custom Templates the Pros Use.

Have questions about a/b testing hypotheses, what is a hypothesis.

Many people define a hypothesis as an “educated guess”.

To be more precise, a properly constructed hypothesis predicts a possible outcome to an experiment or a test where one variable (the independent one ) is tweaked and/or modified and the impact is measured by the change in behavior of another variable (generally the dependent one).

A hypothesis should be specific (it should clearly define what is being altered and what is the expected impact), data-driven (the changes being made to the independent variable should be based on historic data or theories that have been proven in the past), and testable (it should be possible to conduct the proposed test in a controlled environment to establish the relationship between the variables involved, and disprove the hypothesis - should it be untrue.)

What is the Cost of a Hastily Assembled Hypothesis?

According to an analysis of over 28,000 tests run using the Convert Experiences platform, only 1 in 5 tests proves to be statistically significant.

While more and more debate is opening up around sticking to the concept of 95% statistical significance, it is still a valid rule of thumb for optimizers who do not want to get into the fray with peeking vs. no peeking, and custom stopping rules for experiments.

There might be a multitude of reasons why a test does not reach statistical significance. But framing a tenable hypothesis that already proves itself logistically feasible on paper is a better starting point than a hastily assembled assumption.

Moreover, the aim of an A/B test may be to extract a learning, but some learnings come with heavy costs. 26% decrease in conversion rates to be specific.

A robust hypothesis may not be the answer to all testing woes, but it does help prioritisation of possible solutions and leads testing teams to pick low hanging fruits.

How is an A/B Testing Hypothesis Different?

An A/B test should be treated with the same rigour as tests conducted in laboratories. That is an easy way to guarantee better hypotheses, more relevant experiments, and ultimately more profitable optimization programs.

The focus of an A/B test should be on first extracting a learning , and then monetizing it in the form of increased registration completions, better cart conversions and more revenue.

If that is true, then an A/B test hypothesis is not very different from a regular scientific hypothesis. With a couple of interesting points to note:

  • Most scientific hypotheses proceed with one independent variable and one dependent variable, for the sake of simplicity. But in A/B tests, there might be changes made to several independent variables at the same time. Under such circumstances it is good to explore the relationship between the independent variables to make sure that they do not inadvertently impact one another. For example changing both the value proposition and button copy of a landing page to determine improvement in click through or completion rates is tricky. Reaching a point where the browser is compelled to click the button could easily have been impacted by the value proposition (as in a strong hook and heading). So what caused the improvement in the dependent variable? Was it the change to the first element or the second one?
  • The concept of Operational Definition is non-negotiable in most laboratory experiments. And comes baked with the question of ethics or morality. Operation Definition is the specific process that will be used to quantify the change in the value/behavior of the independent variable in the test. As an example, if a test wishes to measure the level of frustration that subjects experience when they are exposed to certain stimuli, researchers must be careful to define exactly how they will measure the output or frustration. Should they allow the test subjects to act out, in which case they may hurt or harm other individuals. Or should they use a non-invasive technique like an fMRI scan to monitor brain activity and collect the needed data. In A/B tests however, since data is collected through relatively inanimate channels like analytics dashboards, generally little thought is spared to Operational Definition and the impact of A/B testing on the human subjects (site traffic in this case).

The 5 Essential Parts of an A/B Testing Hypothesis

A robust A/B testing hypothesis should be assembled in 5 key parts:

Observation stage

1. OBSERVATION

This includes a clear outline of the problem (the unexplained phenomenon) observed and what it entails. This section should be completely free of conjecture and rely solely on good quality data - either qualitative and/or quantitative - to bring a potential area of improvement to light. It also includes a mention of the way in which the data is collected.

Proper observation ensures a credible hypothesis that is easy to “defend” later down the line.

Execution Stage

2. EXECUTION

This is the where, what, and the who of the A/B test. It specifies the change(s) you will be making to site element(s) in an attempt to solve the problem that has been outlined under “OBSERVATION”. It serves to also clearly define the segment of site traffic that will be exposed to the experiment.

Proper execution guidelines set the rhythm for the A/B test. They define how easy or difficult it will be to deploy the test and thus aid hypothesis prioritization .

Logistics Stage

This is where you make your educated guess or informed prediction. Based on a diligently identified OBSERVATION and EXECUTION guidelines that are possible to deploy, your OUTCOME should clearly mention two things:

  • The change (increase or decrease) you expect to see to the problem or the symptoms of the problem identified under OBSERVATION.
  • The Key Performance Indicators (KPIs) you will be monitoring to gauge whether your prediction has panned out, or not.

In general most A/B tests have one primary KPI and a couple of secondary KPIs or ways to measure impact. This is to ensure that external influences do not skew A/B test results and even if the primary KPI is compromised in some way, the secondary KPIs do a good job of indicating that the change is indeed due to the implementation of the EXECUTION guidelines, and not the result of unmonitored external factors.

Logistics Stage

4. LOGISTICS

An important part of hypothesis formulation, LOGISTICS talk about what it will take to collect enough clean data from which a reliable conclusion can be drawn. How many unique tested visitors, what is the statistical significance desired, how many conversions is enough and what is the duration for which the A/B test should run? Each question on its own merits a blog or a lesson. But for the sake of convenience, Convert has created a Free Sample Size & A/B/N Test Duration Calculator .

Set the right logistical expectations so that you can prioritise your hypotheses for maximum impact and minimum effort .

Inadvertent Impact Stage

5. INADVERTENT IMPACT

This is a nod in the direction of ethics in A/B testing and marketing, because experiments involve humans and optimizers should be aware of the possible impact on their behavior.

Often a thorough analysis at this stage can modify the way impact is measured or an experiment is conducted. Or Convert certainly hopes that this will be the case in future. Here’s why ethics do matter in testing.

Now Organize, Prioritise & Learn from Your Hypotheses.

Try convert experiences in free trial & access compass beta - our hypothesis management platform., always working to improve outcomes..

Start Your 15 -Day Free Trial Right Now. No Credit Card Required

Important. Please Read.

  • Check your inbox for the password to Convert’s trial account.
  • Log in using the link provided in that email.

This sign up flow is built for maximum security. You’re worth it!

Grad Coach

Research Aims, Objectives & Questions

The “Golden Thread” Explained Simply (+ Examples)

By: David Phair (PhD) and Alexandra Shaeffer (PhD) | June 2022

The research aims , objectives and research questions (collectively called the “golden thread”) are arguably the most important thing you need to get right when you’re crafting a research proposal , dissertation or thesis . We receive questions almost every day about this “holy trinity” of research and there’s certainly a lot of confusion out there, so we’ve crafted this post to help you navigate your way through the fog.

Overview: The Golden Thread

  • What is the golden thread
  • What are research aims ( examples )
  • What are research objectives ( examples )
  • What are research questions ( examples )
  • The importance of alignment in the golden thread

What is the “golden thread”?  

The golden thread simply refers to the collective research aims , research objectives , and research questions for any given project (i.e., a dissertation, thesis, or research paper ). These three elements are bundled together because it’s extremely important that they align with each other, and that the entire research project aligns with them.

Importantly, the golden thread needs to weave its way through the entirety of any research project , from start to end. In other words, it needs to be very clearly defined right at the beginning of the project (the topic ideation and proposal stage) and it needs to inform almost every decision throughout the rest of the project. For example, your research design and methodology will be heavily influenced by the golden thread (we’ll explain this in more detail later), as well as your literature review.

The research aims, objectives and research questions (the golden thread) define the focus and scope ( the delimitations ) of your research project. In other words, they help ringfence your dissertation or thesis to a relatively narrow domain, so that you can “go deep” and really dig into a specific problem or opportunity. They also help keep you on track , as they act as a litmus test for relevance. In other words, if you’re ever unsure whether to include something in your document, simply ask yourself the question, “does this contribute toward my research aims, objectives or questions?”. If it doesn’t, chances are you can drop it.

Alright, enough of the fluffy, conceptual stuff. Let’s get down to business and look at what exactly the research aims, objectives and questions are and outline a few examples to bring these concepts to life.

Free Webinar: How To Find A Dissertation Research Topic

Research Aims: What are they?

Simply put, the research aim(s) is a statement that reflects the broad overarching goal (s) of the research project. Research aims are fairly high-level (low resolution) as they outline the general direction of the research and what it’s trying to achieve .

Research Aims: Examples  

True to the name, research aims usually start with the wording “this research aims to…”, “this research seeks to…”, and so on. For example:

“This research aims to explore employee experiences of digital transformation in retail HR.”   “This study sets out to assess the interaction between student support and self-care on well-being in engineering graduate students”  

As you can see, these research aims provide a high-level description of what the study is about and what it seeks to achieve. They’re not hyper-specific or action-oriented, but they’re clear about what the study’s focus is and what is being investigated.

Need a helping hand?

converting research questions into hypothesis

Research Objectives: What are they?

The research objectives take the research aims and make them more practical and actionable . In other words, the research objectives showcase the steps that the researcher will take to achieve the research aims.

The research objectives need to be far more specific (higher resolution) and actionable than the research aims. In fact, it’s always a good idea to craft your research objectives using the “SMART” criteria. In other words, they should be specific, measurable, achievable, relevant and time-bound”.

Research Objectives: Examples  

Let’s look at two examples of research objectives. We’ll stick with the topic and research aims we mentioned previously.  

For the digital transformation topic:

To observe the retail HR employees throughout the digital transformation. To assess employee perceptions of digital transformation in retail HR. To identify the barriers and facilitators of digital transformation in retail HR.

And for the student wellness topic:

To determine whether student self-care predicts the well-being score of engineering graduate students. To determine whether student support predicts the well-being score of engineering students. To assess the interaction between student self-care and student support when predicting well-being in engineering graduate students.

  As you can see, these research objectives clearly align with the previously mentioned research aims and effectively translate the low-resolution aims into (comparatively) higher-resolution objectives and action points . They give the research project a clear focus and present something that resembles a research-based “to-do” list.

The research objectives detail the specific steps that you, as the researcher, will take to achieve the research aims you laid out.

Research Questions: What are they?

Finally, we arrive at the all-important research questions. The research questions are, as the name suggests, the key questions that your study will seek to answer . Simply put, they are the core purpose of your dissertation, thesis, or research project. You’ll present them at the beginning of your document (either in the introduction chapter or literature review chapter) and you’ll answer them at the end of your document (typically in the discussion and conclusion chapters).  

The research questions will be the driving force throughout the research process. For example, in the literature review chapter, you’ll assess the relevance of any given resource based on whether it helps you move towards answering your research questions. Similarly, your methodology and research design will be heavily influenced by the nature of your research questions. For instance, research questions that are exploratory in nature will usually make use of a qualitative approach, whereas questions that relate to measurement or relationship testing will make use of a quantitative approach.  

Let’s look at some examples of research questions to make this more tangible.

Research Questions: Examples  

Again, we’ll stick with the research aims and research objectives we mentioned previously.  

For the digital transformation topic (which would be qualitative in nature):

How do employees perceive digital transformation in retail HR? What are the barriers and facilitators of digital transformation in retail HR?  

And for the student wellness topic (which would be quantitative in nature):

Does student self-care predict the well-being scores of engineering graduate students? Does student support predict the well-being scores of engineering students? Do student self-care and student support interact when predicting well-being in engineering graduate students?  

You’ll probably notice that there’s quite a formulaic approach to this. In other words, the research questions are basically the research objectives “converted” into question format. While that is true most of the time, it’s not always the case. For example, the first research objective for the digital transformation topic was more or less a step on the path toward the other objectives, and as such, it didn’t warrant its own research question.  

So, don’t rush your research questions and sloppily reword your objectives as questions. Carefully think about what exactly you’re trying to achieve (i.e. your research aim) and the objectives you’ve set out, then craft a set of well-aligned research questions . Also, keep in mind that this can be a somewhat iterative process , where you go back and tweak research objectives and aims to ensure tight alignment throughout the golden thread.

The importance of strong alignment 

Alignment is the keyword here and we have to stress its importance . Simply put, you need to make sure that there is a very tight alignment between all three pieces of the golden thread. If your research aims and research questions don’t align, for example, your project will be pulling in different directions and will lack focus . This is a common problem students face and can cause many headaches (and tears), so be warned.

Take the time to carefully craft your research aims, objectives and research questions before you run off down the research path. Ideally, get your research supervisor/advisor to review and comment on your golden thread before you invest significant time into your project, and certainly before you start collecting data .  

Recap: The golden thread

In this post, we unpacked the golden thread of research, consisting of the research aims , research objectives and research questions . You can jump back to any section using the links below.

As always, feel free to leave a comment below – we always love to hear from you. Also, if you’re interested in 1-on-1 support, take a look at our private coaching service here.

converting research questions into hypothesis

Psst… there’s more (for free)

This post is part of our dissertation mini-course, which covers everything you need to get started with your dissertation, thesis or research project. 

You Might Also Like:

Narrative analysis explainer

38 Comments

Isaac Levi

Thank you very much for your great effort put. As an Undergraduate taking Demographic Research & Methodology, I’ve been trying so hard to understand clearly what is a Research Question, Research Aim and the Objectives in a research and the relationship between them etc. But as for now I’m thankful that you’ve solved my problem.

Hatimu Bah

Well appreciated. This has helped me greatly in doing my dissertation.

Dr. Abdallah Kheri

An so delighted with this wonderful information thank you a lot.

so impressive i have benefited a lot looking forward to learn more on research.

Ekwunife, Chukwunonso Onyeka Steve

I am very happy to have carefully gone through this well researched article.

Infact,I used to be phobia about anything research, because of my poor understanding of the concepts.

Now,I get to know that my research question is the same as my research objective(s) rephrased in question format.

I please I would need a follow up on the subject,as I intends to join the team of researchers. Thanks once again.

Tosin

Thanks so much. This was really helpful.

Ishmael

I know you pepole have tried to break things into more understandable and easy format. And God bless you. Keep it up

sylas

i found this document so useful towards my study in research methods. thanks so much.

Michael L. Andrion

This is my 2nd read topic in your course and I should commend the simplified explanations of each part. I’m beginning to understand and absorb the use of each part of a dissertation/thesis. I’ll keep on reading your free course and might be able to avail the training course! Kudos!

Scarlett

Thank you! Better put that my lecture and helped to easily understand the basics which I feel often get brushed over when beginning dissertation work.

Enoch Tindiwegi

This is quite helpful. I like how the Golden thread has been explained and the needed alignment.

Sora Dido Boru

This is quite helpful. I really appreciate!

Chulyork

The article made it simple for researcher students to differentiate between three concepts.

Afowosire Wasiu Adekunle

Very innovative and educational in approach to conducting research.

Sàlihu Abubakar Dayyabu

I am very impressed with all these terminology, as I am a fresh student for post graduate, I am highly guided and I promised to continue making consultation when the need arise. Thanks a lot.

Mohammed Shamsudeen

A very helpful piece. thanks, I really appreciate it .

Sonam Jyrwa

Very well explained, and it might be helpful to many people like me.

JB

Wish i had found this (and other) resource(s) at the beginning of my PhD journey… not in my writing up year… 😩 Anyways… just a quick question as i’m having some issues ordering my “golden thread”…. does it matter in what order you mention them? i.e., is it always first aims, then objectives, and finally the questions? or can you first mention the research questions and then the aims and objectives?

UN

Thank you for a very simple explanation that builds upon the concepts in a very logical manner. Just prior to this, I read the research hypothesis article, which was equally very good. This met my primary objective.

My secondary objective was to understand the difference between research questions and research hypothesis, and in which context to use which one. However, I am still not clear on this. Can you kindly please guide?

Derek Jansen

In research, a research question is a clear and specific inquiry that the researcher wants to answer, while a research hypothesis is a tentative statement or prediction about the relationship between variables or the expected outcome of the study. Research questions are broader and guide the overall study, while hypotheses are specific and testable statements used in quantitative research. Research questions identify the problem, while hypotheses provide a focus for testing in the study.

Saen Fanai

Exactly what I need in this research journey, I look forward to more of your coaching videos.

Abubakar Rofiat Opeyemi

This helped a lot. Thanks so much for the effort put into explaining it.

Lamin Tarawally

What data source in writing dissertation/Thesis requires?

What is data source covers when writing dessertation/thesis

Latifat Muhammed

This is quite useful thanks

Yetunde

I’m excited and thankful. I got so much value which will help me progress in my thesis.

Amer Al-Rashid

where are the locations of the reserch statement, research objective and research question in a reserach paper? Can you write an ouline that defines their places in the researh paper?

Webby

Very helpful and important tips on Aims, Objectives and Questions.

Refiloe Raselane

Thank you so much for making research aim, research objectives and research question so clear. This will be helpful to me as i continue with my thesis.

Annabelle Roda-Dafielmoto

Thanks much for this content. I learned a lot. And I am inspired to learn more. I am still struggling with my preparation for dissertation outline/proposal. But I consistently follow contents and tutorials and the new FB of GRAD Coach. Hope to really become confident in writing my dissertation and successfully defend it.

Joe

As a researcher and lecturer, I find splitting research goals into research aims, objectives, and questions is unnecessarily bureaucratic and confusing for students. For most biomedical research projects, including ‘real research’, 1-3 research questions will suffice (numbers may differ by discipline).

Abdella

Awesome! Very important resources and presented in an informative way to easily understand the golden thread. Indeed, thank you so much.

Sheikh

Well explained

New Growth Care Group

The blog article on research aims, objectives, and questions by Grad Coach is a clear and insightful guide that aligns with my experiences in academic research. The article effectively breaks down the often complex concepts of research aims and objectives, providing a straightforward and accessible explanation. Drawing from my own research endeavors, I appreciate the practical tips offered, such as the need for specificity and clarity when formulating research questions. The article serves as a valuable resource for students and researchers, offering a concise roadmap for crafting well-defined research goals and objectives. Whether you’re a novice or an experienced researcher, this article provides practical insights that contribute to the foundational aspects of a successful research endeavor.

yaikobe

A great thanks for you. it is really amazing explanation. I grasp a lot and one step up to research knowledge.

UMAR SALEH

I really found these tips helpful. Thank you very much Grad Coach.

Rahma D.

I found this article helpful. Thanks for sharing this.

Submit a Comment Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

  • Print Friendly

converting research questions into hypothesis

Hypothesis Maker

Ai-powered research hypothesis generator.

  • Scientific Research: Generate a hypothesis for your experimental or observational study based on your research question.
  • Academic Studies: Formulate a hypothesis for your thesis, dissertation, or academic paper.
  • Market Research: Develop a hypothesis for your market research study to understand consumer behavior or market trends.
  • Social Science Research: Create a hypothesis for your social science research to explore societal or behavioral patterns.

New & Trending Tools

Ai journalist, text style transfer, one-page report writer.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Secondary Analysis of Electronic Health Records [Internet]. Cham (CH): Springer; 2016. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-43742-2_9

Cover of Secondary Analysis of Electronic Health Records

Secondary Analysis of Electronic Health Records [Internet].

Chapter 9 formulating the research question.

Anuj Mehta , Brian Malley , and Allan Walkey .

Affiliations

Published online: September 10, 2016.

In this chapter, the reader will learn how to convert a clinical question into a pertinent research question, which includes defining an appropriate study design, select a population sample, the exposure and outcome of interest.

Learning Objectives

  • Understand how to turn a clinical question into a research question.
  • Principles of choosing a sample.
  • Approaches and potential pitfalls.
  • Principles of defining the exposure of interest.
  • Principles of defining the outcome.
  • Selecting an appropriate study design.

9.1. Introduction

The clinical question arising at the time of most health-care decisions is: “will this help my patient?” Before embarking on an investigation to provide data that may be used to inform the clinical question, the question must be modified into a research query. The process of developing a research question involves defining several components of the study and also what type of study is most suited to utilize these components to yield valid and reliable results. These components include: in whom is this research question relevant? The population of subjects defined by the researcher is referred to as the sample. The drug, maneuver, event or characteristic that we are basing our alternative hypothesis on is called the exposure of interest. Finally, the outcome of interest must be defined. With these components in mind the researcher must decide which study design is best or most feasible for answering the question. If an observational study design is chosen, then the choice of a database is also crucial.

In this chapter, we will explore how researchers might work through converting a clinical question into a research question using the clinical scenario of indwelling arterial catheters (IAC) use during mechanical ventilation (MV). Furthermore, we will discuss the strengths and weaknesses of common study designs including randomized controlled trials as well as observational studies.

9.2. The Clinical Scenario: Impact of Indwelling Arterial Catheters

Patients who require MV because they are unable to maintain adequate breathing on their own (e.g. from severe pneumonia or asthma attack) are often the sickest patients in the hospital, with mortality rates exceeding 30 % [ 1 – 3 ]. Multiple options are available to monitor the adequacy of respiratory support for critically ill patients requiring MV, ranging from non-invasive trans-cutaneous measures to invasive, indwelling monitoring systems. IACs are invasive monitoring devices that allow continuous real time blood pressure monitoring and facilitate access to arterial blood sampling to assess arterial blood pH, oxygen and carbon dioxide levels, among others [ 4 – 6 ]. While closer monitoring of patients requiring MV with IACs may appear at face value to be beneficial, IACs may result in severe adverse events, including loss of blood flow to the hand and infection [ 7 , 8 ]. Currently, data is lacking whether benefits may outweigh risks of more intensive monitoring using IACs. Examining factors associated with the decision to use IACs, and outcomes in patients provided IACs as compared to non-invasive monitors alone, may provide information useful to clinicians facing the decision as to whether to place an IAC.

9.3. Turning Clinical Questions into Research Questions

The first step in the process of transforming a clinical question into research is to carefully define the study sample (or patient cohort) , the exposure of interest, and the outcome of interest. These 3 components—sample, exposure, and outcome—are essential parts of every research question. Slight variations in each component can dramatically affect the conclusions that can be drawn from any research study, and whether the research will appropriately address the overarching clinical question.

9.3.1. Study Sample

In the case of IAC use, one might imagine many potential study samples of interest: for example, one might include all ICU patients, all patients receiving MV, all patients receiving intravenous medications that strongly affect blood pressure, adults only, children only, etc. Alternatively, one could define samples based on specific diseases or syndrome, such as shock (where IACs may be used to closely monitor blood pressure) or severe asthma (where IAC may be used to monitor oxygen or carbon dioxide levels).

The choice of study sample will affect both the internal and the external validity (generalizability) of the study. A study focusing only on a pediatric population may not apply to the adult population. Similarly, a study focused on patients receiving MV may not be applicable to non-ventilated patients. Furthermore, a study including patients with different reasons for using an IAC, with different outcomes related to the reason for IAC use, may lack internal validity due to bias called ‘confounding’. Confounding is a type of study bias in which an exposure variable is associated with both the exposure and the outcome.

For instance, if the benefits of IACs on mortality are studied in all patients receiving MV, researchers must take into account the fact that IAC placement may actually be indicative of greater severity of illness. For example, imagine a study with a sample of MV patients in which those with septic shock received an IAC to facilitate vasoactive medications and provide close blood pressuring monitoring while patients with asthma did not receive an IAC as other methods were used to monitor their ventilation (such as end-tidal CO 2 monitoring). Patients with septic shock tend to have a much higher severity of illness compared to patients with asthma regardless of whether an IAC is placed. In such a study, researchers may conclude that IACs are associated with higher mortality only because IACs were used in sicker patients with a higher risk of dying. The variable “diagnosis” is therefore a confounding factor, associated with both the exposure (decision to insert an IAC) and the outcome (death). Careful sample selection is one method of attempting to address issues of confounding related to severity of illness. Restricting study samples to exclude groups that may strongly confound results (i.e. no patients on vasoactive medications) is one strategy to reduce bias. However, the selection of homogeneous study samples to increase internal validity should be balanced with the desire to generalize study findings to broader patient populations. These principles are discussed more extensively in the Chap.  10 —“Cohort Selection”.

9.3.2. Exposure

The exposure in our research question appears to be fairly clear: placement of an IAC. However, careful attention should be paid as to how each exposure or variable of interest is defined. Misclassifying exposures may bias results. How should IAC be measured? For example, investigators may use methods ranging from direct review of the medical chart to use of administrative claims data (i.e. International Classification of Diseases—ICD-codes) to identify IAC use. Each method of ascertaining the exposure of interest may have pros (improved accuracy of medical chart review) and cons (many person-hours to perform manual chart review).

Defining the time window during which an exposure of interest is measured may also have substantial implications that must be considered when interpreting the research results. For the purposes of our IAC study, the presence of an IAC was defined as having an IAC placed after the initiation of MV. The time-dependent nature of the exposure is critical for answering the clinical question; some IACs placed prior to MV are for monitoring of low-risk surgical patients in the operating room. Including all patients with IACs regardless of timing may bias the results towards a benefit for IACs by including many otherwise healthy patients who had an IAC placed for surgical monitoring. Alternatively, if the exposure group is defined as patients who had an IAC at least 48 h after initiation of MV, the study is at risk for a type of confounding called “immortal time bias”: only patients who were alive could have had an IAC placed, whereas patients dying prior to 48 h (supposedly sicker) could not have had an IAC.

Equally important to defining the group of patients who received or experienced an exposure is to define the “unexposed” or control group. While not all research requires a control group (e.g. epidemiologic studies), a control group is needed to assess the effectiveness of healthcare interventions. In the case of the IAC study, the control group is fairly straightforward: patients receiving MV who did not have an IAC placed. However, there are important nuances when defining control groups. In our study example, an alternate control group could be all ICU patients who did not receive an IAC. However, the inclusion of patients not receiving MV results in a control group with a lower severity of illness and expected mortality than patients receiving MV, which would bias in favor of not using IACs. Careful definition of the control group is needed to properly interpret any conclusions from research; defining an appropriate control group is as important as defining the exposure.

9.3.3. Outcome

Finally, the investigator needs to determine the outcome of interest. Several different types of outcomes can be considered, including intermediate or mechanistic outcomes (informs etiological pathways, but may not immediately impact patients), patient-centered outcomes (informs outcomes important to patients, but may lack mechanistic insights: e.g. comfort scales, quality of life indices, or mortality), or healthcare-system centered outcomes (e.g. resource utilization, or costs). In our example of IAC use, several outcomes could be considered including intermediate outcomes (e.g. number of arterial blood draws, ventilator setting changes, or vasoactive medication changes), patient-centered outcomes (e.g. 28-day or 90-day mortality, adverse event rates), or healthcare utilization (e.g. hospitalization costs, added clinician workload). As shown in our example, outcome(s) may build upon each other to yield a constellation of findings that provides a more complete picture to address the clinical question of interest.

  • “ In the population of interest ( study cohort ), is the exposure to the variable of interest associated with a different outcome than in the control group ? ”, which becomes, in our example:
  • “ Among mechanically ventilated, adult ICU patients who are not receiving vasoactive medications (i.e., the study sample) is placement of an IAC after initiation of MV (as compared with not receiving an IAC) (i.e. the exposure and control patients) associated with improved 28 - day mortality rates (primary outcome, patient - centered) and the number of blood gas measurements per day (supporting secondary outcome, intermediate/mechanistic)? ”

9.4. Matching Study Design to the Research Question

Once the research question has been defined, the next step is to choose the optimal study design given the question and resources available. In biomedical research, the gold-standard for study design remains the double-blinded, randomized, placebo-controlled trial (RCT) [ 9 , 10 ]. In a RCT, patients with a given condition (e.g. all adults receiving MV) would be randomized to receive a drug or intervention of interest (e.g. IAC) or randomized to receive the control (e.g. no IAC), with careful measurement of pre-determined outcomes (e.g. 28-day mortality). In ideal conditions, the randomization process eliminates all measured and unmeasured confounding and allows for causal inferences to be drawn, which cannot generally be achieved without randomization. As shown above, confounding is a threat to valid inferences from study results. Alternatively, in our example of septic shock verses asthma, severity of illness associated with the underlying condition may represent another confounder. Randomization solely based on the exposure of interest attempts to suppress issues of confounding. In our examples, proper randomization in a large sample would theoretically create equal age distributions and equal numbers of patients with septic shock and asthma in both the exposure and the control group.

However, RCTs have several limitations. Although the theoretical underpinnings of RCTs are fairly simple, the complex logistics of patient enrollment and retention, informed consent, randomization, follow up, and blinding may result in RCTs deviating from the ‘ideal conditions’ necessary for unbiased, causal inference. Additionally, RCTs carry the highest potential for patient harm and require intensive monitoring because the study dictates what type of treatment a patient receives (rather than the doctor) and may deviate from routine care. Given the logistic complexity, RCTs are often time- and cost-intensive, frequently taking many years and millions of dollars to complete. Even when logistically feasible, RCTs often ‘weed out’ multiple groups of patients in order to minimize potential harms and maximize detection of associations between interventions and outcomes of interest. As a result, RCTs can consist of homogeneous patients meeting narrow criteria, which may reduce the external validity of the studies’ findings. Despite much effort and cost, an RCT may miss relevance to the clinical question as to whether the intervention of interest is helpful for your particular patient or not. Finally, some clinical questions may not ethically be answered with RCTs. For instance, the link between smoking and lung cancer has never been shown in a RCT, as it is unethical to randomize patients to start smoking in a smoking intervention group, or randomize patients to a control group in a trial to investigate the efficacy of parachutes [ 11 ]!

Observational research differs from RCTs. Observational studies are non-experimental; researchers record routine medical practice patterns and derive conclusions based on correlations and associations without active interventions [ 9 , 12 ]. Observational studies can be retrospective (based on data that has already been collected), prospective (data is actively collected over time), or ambi-directional (a mix). Unlike RCTs, researchers in observational studies have no role in deciding what types of treatments or interventions patients receive. Observational studies tend to be logistically less complicated than RCTs as there is no active intervention, no randomization, no data monitoring boards, and data is often collected retrospectively. As such, observational studies carry less risk of harm to patients (other than loss of confidentiality of data that has been collected) than RCTs, and tend to be less time- and cost-intensive. Retrospective databases like MIMIC-II [ 13 ] or the National Inpatient Sample [ 14 ] can also provide much larger study samples (tens of thousands in some instances) than could be enrolled in an RCT, thus providing larger statistical power. Additionally, broader study samples are often included in observational studies, leading to greater generalizability of the results to a wider range of patients (external validity). Finally, certain clinical questions that would be unethical to study in an RCT can be investigated with observational studies. For example, the link between lung cancer and tobacco use has been demonstrated with multiple large prospective epidemiological studies [ 15 , 16 ] and the life-saving effects of parachutes have been demonstrated mostly through the powers of observation.

Although logistically simpler than RCTs, the theoretical underpinnings of observational studies are generally more complex than RCTs. Obtaining causal estimates of the effect of a specific exposure on a specific outcome depends on the philosophical concept of the ‘counterfactual’ [ 17 ]. The counterfactual is the situation in which, all being equal, the same research subject at the same time would receive the exposure of interest and (the counterfactual) not receive the exposure of interest, with the same outcome measured in the exposed and unexposed research subject. Because we cannot create cloned research subjects in the real-world, we rely on creating groups of patients similar to the group that receives an intervention of interest. In the case of an ideal RCT with a large enough number of subjects, the randomization process used to select the intervention and control groups creates two alternate ‘universes’ of patients that will be similar except as related to the exposure of interest. Because observational studies cannot intervene on study subjects, observational studies create natural experiments in which the counterfactual group is defined by the investigator and by clinical processes occurring in the real-world. Importantly, real-world clinical processes often occur for a reason, and these reasons can cause deviation from counterfactual ideals in which exposed and unexposed study subjects differ in important ways. In short, observational studies may be more prone to bias (problems with internal validity) than RCTs due to difficulty obtaining the counterfactual control group.

Several types of biases have been identified in observational studies. Selection bias occurs when the process of selecting exposed and unexposed patients introduces a bias into the study. For example, the time between starting MV and receiving IAC may introduce a type of “survivor treatment selection bias” since patients who received IAC could not have died prior to receiving IACs. Information bias stems from mismeasurement or misclassification of certain variables. For retrospective studies, the data has already been collected and sometimes it is difficult to evaluate for errors in the data. Another major bias in observational studies is confounding. As stated, confounding occurs when a third variable is correlated with both the exposure and outcome. If the third variable is not taken into consideration, a spurious relationship between the exposure and outcome may be inferred. For example, smoking is an important confounder in several observational studies as it is associated with several other behaviors such as coffee and alcohol consumption. A study investigating the relationship between coffee consumption and incidence of lung cancer may conclude that individuals who drink more coffee have higher rates of lung cancer. However, as smoking is associated with both coffee consumption and lung cancer, it is confounder in the relationship between coffee consumption and lung cancer if unmeasured and unaccounted for in analysis. Several methods have been developed to attempt to address confounding in observational research such as adjusting for the confounder in regression equations if it is known and measured, matching cohorts by known confounders, and using instrumental variables—methods that will be explained in-depth in future chapters. Alternatively, one can restrict the study sample (e.g. excluding patients with shock from a study evaluating the utility of IACs). For these reasons, while powerful, an individual observational study can, at best, demonstrate associations and correlations and cannot prove causation. Over time, a cumulative sum of multiple high quality observational studies coupled with other mechanistic evidence can lead to causal conclusions, such as in the causal link currently accepted between smoking and lung cancer established by observational human studies and experimental trials in animals.

9.5. Types of Observational Research

There are multiple different types of questions that can be answered with observational research (Table  9.1 ). Epidemiological studies are one major type of observational research that focuses on the burden of disease in predefined populations. These types of studies often attempt to define incidence, prevalence, and risk factors for disease. Additionally, epidemiological studies also can investigate changes to healthcare or diseases over time. Epidemiological studies are the cornerstone of public health and can heavily influence policy decisions, resource allocation, and patient care. In the case of lung cancer, predefined groups of patients without lung cancer were monitored for years until some patients developed lung cancer. Researchers then compared numerous risk factors, like smoking, between those who did and did not develop lung cancer which led to the conclusion that smoking increased the risk of lung cancer [ 15 , 16 ].

Table 9.1

Table 9.1

Major types of observational research, and their purpose

There are other types of epidemiological studies that are based on similar principles of observational research but differ in the types of questions posed. Predictive modeling studies develop models that are able to accurately predict future outcomes in specific groups of patients. In predictive studies, researchers define an outcome of interest (e.g. hospital mortality) and use data collected on patients such as labs, vital signs, and disease states to determine which factors contributed to the outcome. Researchers then validate the models developed from one group of patients in a separate group of patients. Predictive modeling studies developed many common prediction scores used in clinical practice such as the Framingham Cardiovascular Risk Score [ 18 ], APACHE IV [ 19 ], SAPS II [ 20 ], and SOFA [ 21 ].

Comparative effectiveness research is another form of observational research which involves the comparison of existing healthcare interventions in order to determine effective methods to deliver healthcare. Unlike descriptive epidemiologic studies, comparative effectiveness research compares outcomes between similar patients who received different treatments in order to assess which intervention may be associated with superior outcomes in real-world conditions. This could involve comparing drug A to drug B or could involve comparing one intervention to a control group who did not receive that intervention. Given that there are often underlying reasons why one patient received treatment A versus B or an intervention versus no intervention, comparative effectiveness studies must meticulously account for potential confounding factors. In the case of IACs, the research question comparing patients who had an IAC placed to those who did not have an IAC placed would represent a comparative effectiveness study.

Pharmacovigilance studies are yet another form of observational research. As many drug and device trials end after 1 or 2 years, observational methods are used to evaluate if there are patterns of rarer adverse events occurring in the long-term. Phase IV clinical studies are one form of pharmacovigilance studies in which long-term information related to efficacy and harm are gathered after the drug has been approved.

9.6. Choosing the Right Database

A critical part of the research process is deciding what types of data are needed to answer the research question. Administrative/claims data, secondary use of clinical trial data, prospective epidemiologic studies, and electronic health record (EHR) systems (both from individual institutions and those pooled from multiple institutions) are several sources from which databases can be built. Administrative or claims databases, such as the National Inpatient Sample and State Inpatient Databases complied by the Healthcare Cost and Utilization Project or the Medicare database, contain information on patient and hospital demographics as well as billing and procedure codes. Several techniques have been developed to translate these billing and procedure codes to more clinically useful disease descriptions. Administrative databases tend to provide very large sample sizes and, in some cases, can be representative of an entire population. However, they lack granular patient-level data from the hospitalization such as vital signs, laboratory and microbiology data, timing data (such as duration of MV or days with an IAC) or pharmacology data, which are often important in dealing with possible confounders.

Another common source of data for observational research is large epidemiologic studies like the Framingham Heart Study as well as large multicenter RCTs such as the NIH ARDS Network. Data that has already been can be analyzed retrospectively with new research questions in mind. As the original data was collected for research purposes, these types of databases often have detailed, granular information not available in other clinical databases. However, researchers are often bound by the scope of data collection from the original research study which limits the questions that may be posed. Importantly, generalizability may be limited in data from trials.

The advent of Electronic Health Records (EHR) has resulted in the digitization of medical records from their prior paper format. The resulting digitized medical records present opportunities to overcome some of the shortcomings of administrative data, yielding granular data with laboratory results, medications, and timing of clinical events [ 13 ]. These “big databases” take advantage of the fact many EHRs collect data from a variety of sources such as patient monitors, laboratory systems, and pharmacy systems and coalesce them into one system for clinicians. This information can then be translated into de-identified databases for research purposes that contain detailed patient demographics, billing and procedure information, timing data, hospital outcomes data, as well as patient-level granular data and provider notes which can searched using natural language processing tools. “Big data” approaches may attenuate confounding by providing detailed information needed to assess severity of illness (such as lab results and vital signs). Furthermore, the granular nature of the data can provide insight as to the reason why one patient received an intervention and another did not which can partly address confounding by indication. Thus, the promise of “big data” is that it contains small, very detailed data. “Big data” databases, such as MIMIC-III, have the potential to expand the scope of what had previously been possible with observational research.

9.7. Putting It Together

Fewer than 10 % of clinical decisions are supported by high level evidence [ 22 ]. Clinical questions arise approximately in every other patient [ 23 ] and provide a large cache of research questions. When formulating a research question, investigators must carefully select the appropriate sample of subjects, exposure variable, outcome variable, and confounding variables. Once the research question is clear, study design becomes the next pivotal step. While RCTs are the gold standard for establishing causal inference under ideal conditions, they are not always practical, cost-effective, ethical or even possible for some types of questions. Observational research presents an alternative to performing RCTs, but is often limited in causal inference by unmeasured confounding.

Our clinical scenario gave rise to the question of whether IACs improved the outcomes of patients receiving MV. This translated into the research question: “Among mechanically ventilated ICU patients not receiving vasoactive medications (study sample) is use of an IAC after initiation of MV (exposure) associated with improved 28-day mortality (outcome)?” While an RCT could answer this question, it would be logistically complex, costly, and difficult. Using comparative effectiveness techniques, one can pose the question using a granular retrospective database comparing patients who received an IAC to measurably similar patients who did not have an IAC placed. However, careful attention must be paid to unmeasured confounding by indication as to why some patients received IAC and others did not. Factors such as severity of illness, etiology of respiratory failure, and presence of certain diseases that make IAC placement difficult (such as peripheral arterial disease) may be considered as possible confounders of the association between IAC and mortality. While an administrative database could be used, it could lack important information related to possible confounders. As such, EHR databases like MIMIC-III, with detailed granular patient-level data, may allow for measurement of a greater number of previously unmeasured confounding variables and allow for greater attenuation of bias in observational research.

Take Home Messages

  • Most research questions arise from clinical scenarios in which the proper course of treatment is unclear or unknown.
  • Defining a research question requires careful consideration of the optimal study sample, exposure, and outcome in order to answer a clinical question of interest.
  • While observational research studies can overcome many of the limitations of randomized controlled trials, careful consideration of study design and database selection is needed to address bias and confounding.

Open Access    This chapter is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ ), which permits any noncommercial use, duplication, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, a link is provided to the Creative Commons license and any changes made are indicated.

The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the work’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if such material is not included in the work’s Creative Commons license and the respective action is not permitted by statutory regulation, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to duplicate, adapt or reproduce the material.

  • Cite this Page Mehta A, Malley B, Walkey A. Formulating the Research Question. 2016 Sep 10. In: Secondary Analysis of Electronic Health Records [Internet]. Cham (CH): Springer; 2016. Chapter 9. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-43742-2_9
  • PDF version of this page (122K)
  • PDF version of this title (15M)

In this Page

  • Introduction
  • The Clinical Scenario: Impact of Indwelling Arterial Catheters
  • Turning Clinical Questions into Research Questions
  • Matching Study Design to the Research Question
  • Types of Observational Research
  • Choosing the Right Database
  • Putting It Together

Similar articles in PubMed

  • Review Defining the Patient Cohort. [Secondary Analysis of Electron...] Review Defining the Patient Cohort. Moskowitz A, Chen K. Secondary Analysis of Electronic Health Records. 2016
  • Procedures and methods of benefit assessments for medicines in Germany. [Eur J Health Econ. 2008] Procedures and methods of benefit assessments for medicines in Germany. Bekkering GE, Kleijnen J. Eur J Health Econ. 2008 Nov; 9 Suppl 1:5-29.
  • [Procedures and methods of benefit assessments for medicines in Germany]. [Dtsch Med Wochenschr. 2008] [Procedures and methods of benefit assessments for medicines in Germany]. Bekkering GE, Kleijnen J. Dtsch Med Wochenschr. 2008 Dec; 133 Suppl 7:S225-46. Epub 2008 Nov 25.
  • Defining the primary research question in veterinary clinical studies. [J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2016] Defining the primary research question in veterinary clinical studies. Giuffrida MA. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2016 Sep 1; 249(5):547-51.
  • Review Formulating questions and locating primary studies for inclusion in systematic reviews. [Ann Intern Med. 1997] Review Formulating questions and locating primary studies for inclusion in systematic reviews. Counsell C. Ann Intern Med. 1997 Sep 1; 127(5):380-7.

Recent Activity

  • Formulating the Research Question - Secondary Analysis of Electronic Health Reco... Formulating the Research Question - Secondary Analysis of Electronic Health Records

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

Research Skills Tutorial

  • Turning A Topic Into A Research Question
  • Getting Background Information About Your Topic
  • Portfolio Activity 1
  • Types of Information Sources
  • Scholarly, Popular and Trade Information Sources
  • Primary, Secondary and Tertiary Information Sources
  • Current and Retrospective Information Sources
  • Information Sources for Different Audiences and Purposes
  • Portfolio Activity 2
  • Library Databases vs. the Web
  • Creating a Concept Chart
  • Boolean Operators
  • Refining Results by Date, Peer Review and Document Type
  • Advanced Searching
  • Advanced Searching with Descriptors
  • Descriptor Chaining
  • Citation Chaining (or Reference Mining)
  • Searching Outside the SUNY Empire Library
  • Searching Google
  • Portfolio Activity 3
  • Understanding Search Results
  • Saving Search Results
  • Portfolio Activity 4
  • Credible Sources
  • What Does the Information Source Tell You About Itself?
  • Reliability
  • Some More About Validity
  • Portfolio Activity 5
  • What Is Citing?
  • What Is a Citation?
  • Citation Styles
  • Citing Tools and Tips
  • Practice Activity 6
  • Practice Activity 7 - Final Project

Turn A Topic Into A Research Question

Choose your starting research topic.

When you get an assignment where you need to choose your own topic, begin by considering topics covered in your course and textbooks/readings that fit the assignment. Then do a some background research (covered on next page) on one or more of those topics to get a a bird's eye historical view. This will not only help you narrow your focus, but equip you with the necessary vocabulary (names of people, places and things related to the topic) to search the scholarly literature. 

Let's look at a brief example of how this narrowing process might look on paper:

Starting idea: cyberbullying

What things would you need to find out in order to write about this topic? What people ( high school, middle school, or college students? ), places ( U.S., NYS, or a comparison between places? ) and related concepts ( internet trolls, state and federal laws, school policies, social media platforms, statistics, such as number of people who experience or see it? ) are connected to this idea?

Some background readings (click links to explore): Encyclopedia entry , Wikipedia , Gov't site

Ideas for narrowing the topic (freeform brainstorm - look at aspects of the topic from above that appeal to you - put into the form of questions): Cyberbullying and child development? Cyberbullying and post-traumatic stress? Is there a connection between those who troll on the internet and those who engage in cyberbullying? What policies have been put in place in NYS (or my local school district) to combat cyberbullying? What is the relationship, if any, between economic and or ethnic status and cyberbullying?

Locate more information sources related to the topic (books, journal articles, government reports, etc.). Read or skim them and begin to formulate a more concrete direction for your research to take. Take notes . As your knowledge of the topic grows, ideas for what ultimate question(s) your paper will attempt to answer should start taking shape.

The following video explains how to find a balance between too broad and general, and too narrow. That way you are able to find enough research materials to write intelligently about, but aren't overwhelmed. 

Turn your topic into a research question and thesis statement.

Research is a form of problem solving, and the first step in problem solving is always to identify the problem. In other words, what question do you hope to answer with your research?

Once you have your research question, the next step is to reformulate it as a thesis statement. The thesis statement answers your research question in such a way that it leads into the major points that you will make in your paper. 

Accessibility Note

Please note: If you need to request accommodations with content linked to on this guide, on the basis of a disability, please contact Accessibility Resources and Services by emailing them .  Requests for accommodations should be submitted as early as possible to allow for sufficient planning. If you have questions, please visit the Accessibility Resources and Services website .

  • << Previous: Ch. 1 - Developing Research Questions
  • Next: Getting Background Information About Your Topic >>
  • Last Updated: Feb 21, 2024 1:37 PM
  • URL: https://subjectguides.sunyempire.edu/researchskillstutorial

SpringShare Privacy Policy

  • Safety & Security
  • ITS Service Desk
  • Facilities & Maintenance
  • University Policies
  • Web Accessibility
  • Freedom of Information

Facebook

© 2023 SUNY Empire State University The Torch logo is a trademark of SUNY Empire State University.

IMAGES

  1. Research Questions.docx

    converting research questions into hypothesis

  2. PPT

    converting research questions into hypothesis

  3. Research Question vs Hypothesis: how to convert research questions into

    converting research questions into hypothesis

  4. Hypothesis vs Research Questions in Hindi Urdu Lecture 31

    converting research questions into hypothesis

  5. Solved 4. How can you test hypothesis in research? Give

    converting research questions into hypothesis

  6. Research Questions, Hypothesis Variables

    converting research questions into hypothesis

VIDEO

  1. M&DRTW: Conceptualising Research- Formulating Research problems/ research questions/hypothesis

  2. How to frame the Hypothesis statement in your Research

  3. Pract 1

  4. What is Hypothesis

  5. Biostatistics Examination Practice Question for Students (13 of 1000) : Scales of measurement

  6. Biostatistics Examination Practice Question for Students (12 of 1000) : Standard normal distribution

COMMENTS

  1. PDF DEVELOPING HYPOTHESIS AND RESEARCH QUESTIONS

    RESEARCH QUESTIONS. Qualitative Approach. The use of Research Questions as opposed to objectives or hypothesis, is more frequent. Characteristics Use of words- what or how. Specify whether the study: discovers, seeks to understand, explores or describes the experiences. Use of non-directional wording in the question.

  2. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis

    5. Phrase your hypothesis in three ways. To identify the variables, you can write a simple prediction in if…then form. The first part of the sentence states the independent variable and the second part states the dependent variable. If a first-year student starts attending more lectures, then their exam scores will improve.

  3. Research Question vs Hypothesis: how to convert research questions into

    Learn the difference between research questions and hypotheses, and how to convert a research question into a hypothesis.Need research ideas? Get My 30 Day R...

  4. PDF Translating a research question into a testable hypothesis

    Translating a research question into a testable hypothesis The first thing is to make sure your question is simple, as opposed to compound (i.e. one single question that does not have multiple parts). For example: Does milling affect particle size and what is the optimal particle size? is a compound question, it has two parts 1)

  5. A Practical Guide to Writing Quantitative and Qualitative Research

    INTRODUCTION. Scientific research is usually initiated by posing evidenced-based research questions which are then explicitly restated as hypotheses.1,2 The hypotheses provide directions to guide the study, solutions, explanations, and expected results.3,4 Both research questions and hypotheses are essentially formulated based on conventional theories and real-world processes, which allow the ...

  6. Null and Alternative Hypotheses

    Converting research questions to hypothesis is a simple task. Take the questions and make it a positive statement that says a relationship exists (correlation studies) or a difference exists between the groups (experiment study) and you have the alternative hypothesis. Write the statement such that a relationship does not exist or a difference ...

  7. PDF Research Questions and Hypotheses

    Most quantitative research falls into one or more of these three categories. The most rigorous form of quantitative research follows from a test of a theory (see Chapter 3) and the specification of research questions or hypotheses that are included in the theory. The independent and dependent variables must be measured sepa-rately.

  8. A well-formulated research question: The foundation stone of good

    The research question presents an idea to be studied, whereas the hypothesis aims to answer the research question. By converting it into a statement, a research question can be converted into a hypothesis. For example, 'Does a pre-emptive nerve block decrease opioid consumption in nasal surgeries?' is a research question while the ...

  9. Shaping the research question and hypothesis

    The research question and hypothesis mark your own disciplinary territory and drive your research. Whether you have both a research question and a hypothesis, and whether they are broken down into several sub-areas, will depend on your discipline and topic; but however they appear, it is important that they are clearly formulated.

  10. Developing a Hypothesis

    Theories and Hypotheses. Before describing how to develop a hypothesis, it is important to distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis. A theory is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena. Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes ...

  11. Hypothesis Maker

    Our hypothesis maker is a simple and efficient tool you can access online for free. If you want to create a research hypothesis quickly, you should fill out the research details in the given fields on the hypothesis generator. Below are the fields you should complete to generate your hypothesis:

  12. Developing a Hypothesis

    The first is to raise a research question, answer that question by conducting a new study, and then offer one or more theories (usually more) to explain or interpret the results. This format works well for applied research questions and for research questions that existing theories do not address. The second way is to describe one or more ...

  13. How to Write a Hypothesis 101: A Step-by-Step Guide

    Hypothesis: The Foundation of Your Research. A hypothesis is an educated guess about the relationship between two or more variables. It provides the basis for your research question and is a starting point for an experiment or observational study. The critical elements for a hypothesis include: Specificity: A clear and concise statement that ...

  14. Develop Question/Hypothesis

    Step 3: Ask Open-Ended Questions. To generate a research question, ask open-ended questions related to your focused question. Start by asking questions focusing on "what," "why," "how," or "in what way" and answers to these questions will assist you in your exploration and discovery. Brainstorm and list as many questions as possible without ...

  15. Formulating the Research Question and Framing the Hypothesis

    Three steps comprise the formulation of a great research question: (1) ask interesting questions, (2) select the best question for research, and (3) transform the research question into a testable ...

  16. Research questions, hypotheses and objectives

    The development of the research question, including a supportive hypothesis and objectives, is a necessary key step in producing clinically relevant results to be used in evidence-based practice. A well-defined and specific research question is more likely to help guide us in making decisions about study design and population and subsequently ...

  17. Convert Hypothesis Generator: Free Tool for A/B Testers

    Each question on its own merits a blog or a lesson. But for the sake of convenience, Convert has created a Free Sample Size & A/B/N Test Duration Calculator . Set the right logistical expectations so that you can prioritise your hypotheses for maximum impact and minimum effort . 5. INADVERTENT IMPACT.

  18. Research Questions, Objectives & Aims (+ Examples)

    The research aims, objectives and research questions (collectively called the "golden thread") are arguably the most important thing you need to get right when you're crafting a research proposal, dissertation or thesis.We receive questions almost every day about this "holy trinity" of research and there's certainly a lot of confusion out there, so we've crafted this post to help ...

  19. Hypothesis and research questions

    A hypothesis is used to guide the research process and to help researchers design an experiment to test the validity of the hypothesis. A research question is a specific question that a study is ...

  20. Hypothesis Maker

    How to use Hypothesis Maker. Visit the tool's page. Enter your research question into the provided field. Click the 'Generate' button to let the AI generate a hypothesis based on your research question. Review the generated hypothesis and adjust it as necessary to fit your research context and objectives. Copy and paste the hypothesis into your ...

  21. Formulating the Research Question

    Turning Clinical Questions into Research Questions. The first step in the process of transforming a clinical question into research is to carefully define the study sample (or patient cohort), the exposure of interest, and the outcome of interest. These 3 components—sample, exposure, and outcome—are essential parts of every research question.

  22. Turning A Topic Into A Research Question

    The thesis statement answers your research question in such a way that it leads into the major points that you will make in your paper. Accessibility Note Please note: If you need to request accommodations with content linked to on this guide, on the basis of a disability, please contact Accessibility Resources and Services by emailing them .

  23. SCI 100 4-2 Activity Turning Questions Into Hypotheses

    SCI 100 Module Four Activity Template: Turning Questions into Hypotheses. Instructions Write a hypothesis that is directly related to the research question you finalized in the previous module. Use instructor feedback on that activity and the knowledge you have gained from the course resources to assist you.